Podchaser Logo
Home
Evan Thompson: Is Mediation a "mind science?"

Evan Thompson: Is Mediation a "mind science?"

Released Friday, 25th November 2022
Good episode? Give it some love!
Evan Thompson: Is Mediation a "mind science?"

Evan Thompson: Is Mediation a "mind science?"

Evan Thompson: Is Mediation a "mind science?"

Evan Thompson: Is Mediation a "mind science?"

Friday, 25th November 2022
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

one is being told that

0:03

one's doing is practicing a

0:05

science of seeing things as they really

0:07

are. all along the way one is actually

0:09

being given a kind of conceptual framework and

0:11

script for how to understand and

0:13

relate to the mind. that involves a

0:15

kind of, you might say, baby version

0:18

or minimal version of a Buddhist psychology.

0:20

That is a skill. That is a practice.

0:24

But it's not a kind

0:26

of disinterested examination

0:28

of how things are neutrally

0:32

So in saying that it's not a science,

0:34

I don't mean to say that it's anti science

0:36

or opposed to science. I mean to say

0:38

it's just different. and that casting

0:40

it in the rhetoric of science really

0:43

falsifies what it is.

0:49

welcome to brain science, the podcast that

0:51

explores how neuroscience is unraveling

0:53

the mystery of how our brain

0:55

makes us human. I'm your host, Dr.

0:58

Ginger Campbell, and this is episode two hundred

1:00

and two. The title

1:02

of this episode is Is

1:05

meditation a brain science?

1:07

And my guest is

1:09

philosopher Evan Thompson. Obviously,

1:12

The excerpt you just heard is a spoiler

1:15

Ginger Thompson clearly answers no

1:17

to this question. This

1:20

is doctor Thompson third appearance on

1:22

Neuroscience, and he is

1:23

uniquely positioned to answer

1:25

this challenging question.

1:27

Before we jump into the interview,

1:29

I just want to remind

1:30

you of a few things. First,

1:33

you can get complete show notes and episode

1:35

transcripts on our website at

1:37

brain science podcast dot com,

1:39

and you can send me feedback at

1:41

brain science podcast at gmail

1:43

dot com. To get

1:45

Neuroscience show notes automatically

1:47

each month, sign up for the free

1:49

brain science newsletter, at

1:51

brain science podcast dot com or just

1:54

text brain

1:54

science, all one word, 255444

1:59

When you sign

1:59

up, you'll also get a free gift entitled

2:02

five things you need to know about

2:04

your brain. Just text.

2:07

Brain signs all one word to 55444

2:12

Because brain science is independently

2:14

produced, it relies on the support of listeners

2:17

like you. You can learn more at brain

2:19

science dot com forward slash

2:22

donations. I'll be

2:24

back after the interview with a

2:26

summary of the key ideas and

2:28

a few brief announcements.

2:34

Thompson, welcome back to brain science.

2:37

Thank you. I'm glad to be back. I

2:39

was looking back at our old episodes

2:42

and realized that we first talked in

2:44

two thousand and twelve, which is

2:46

obviously ten years ago, and

2:48

we haven't really talked since around

2:50

two thousand fifteen when we talked about your

2:52

other book that I can't remember

2:54

the name of off the top of my head. But anyhow,

2:57

a few months ago when

2:59

I was preparing to release

3:01

your

3:02

original interview from

3:04

ten years ago, That's when I discovered

3:07

that she'd written this book in twenty twenty

3:09

that I had totally missed called why

3:11

I am not a Buddhist. And when

3:13

I read it, it really resonated

3:16

with me because when I first started

3:18

exploring neuroscience back in two thousand

3:20

three, I called myself

3:22

a Buddhist, but the more

3:24

I learned about how the brain really works,

3:27

the more it seemed

3:28

incompatible with

3:30

basic Buddhist ideas.

3:33

But the main reason I invited

3:35

you back is so that we can critically

3:38

examine

3:39

the claim that meditation is

3:43

a mind science. Now,

3:47

some of my listeners are gonna be thinking of

3:49

you as a proponent of embodied especially

3:51

since I just replayed that early interview.

3:54

So perhaps you could start out with

3:57

like you did in this new book

3:59

explaining

3:59

your unique background

4:02

and how it gives you a really

4:05

unusual perspective on this question.

4:08

I guess it depends how far back we go, but

4:10

I was raised as a teenager in

4:13

an alternative educational and

4:16

spiritual institute that was also

4:18

a commune So this is in the nineteen

4:20

seventies that was founded by my parents. It

4:22

was called the Lindisfarne Meditation. And

4:26

my dad had been a university professor

4:29

he quit the university as a tenured full professor

4:31

because he thought that the

4:33

universities really weren't advancing the

4:35

kind of knowledge that he

4:37

thought was needed to deal with

4:39

the times in which we were living. So this is

4:41

the early nineteen seventies and in a way a

4:43

precursor to all of the things we're dealing with now,

4:45

especially the climate crisis. So he thought that

4:47

there needed to be other modes of learning and other

4:49

modes of discussion. So he created this alternative

4:51

institute and he brought together scientists and

4:54

spiritual teachers, religious teachers,

4:56

philosophers, artists, poets, activists.

4:58

It was a kind of a salon in a way

5:01

of the counterculture in the nineteen seventies.

5:03

And it was in that context that I was

5:05

first exposed to Buddhism

5:08

and to meditation. Actually,

5:10

my father taught me meditation when I

5:12

was very young. He had been raised afflic

5:14

but left the church and then discovered yoga

5:17

and Hinduism on his own in the nineteen

5:19

fifties in LA and became a practitioner of

5:21

yoga meditation and taught me that when I was a little

5:23

kid. And so that was my first

5:25

real taste Meditation. But then growing up

5:27

at Lindisfarne, I encountered a lot

5:29

of different religious spiritual

5:31

teachers somewhere turn some Asian engaged

5:34

in conversations with scientists and

5:36

philosophers about the

5:38

mind. And

5:40

that then fueled my interest and propelled

5:42

me into college where

5:45

I majored in Asian studies. So

5:47

I studied Chinese language and Chinese

5:49

history and Asian philosophy. And

5:51

then when I was thinking of going on to

5:54

grad school, I really hit on

5:56

philosophy as the main thing that

5:58

was at the core of my interest. So I, you know, went

6:00

to grad school and got my PhD in philosophy.

6:02

But while I was writing my dissertation,

6:05

which turned out to be in the cognitive

6:07

science and philosophy of color

6:11

I worked in parallel

6:13

with a neuroscientist named Francisco

6:15

Varela, who I had met at Lindisfarne, who

6:18

was a pioneering neuroscientist tests

6:20

known for his work in theoretical biology and

6:22

his work on large scale

6:24

neural assembly, Michael activity,

6:26

and its relationship to cognition and consciousness.

6:29

But he was also a practicing Buddhist

6:31

and had been giving a series

6:33

of lectures on the relationship

6:36

between buddhist philosophy

6:38

and psychology, you could say, and

6:40

his own understanding of the mind and brain as

6:42

a neuroscientist, and he he

6:44

wanted to turn these lectures into a book.

6:46

So he knew that I had an undergraduate background

6:49

in Asian phosphate and Meditation, and

6:51

that I was now studying cognitive science in graduate

6:54

school. So he brought me to

6:56

Paris, which is where his lab was

6:58

at the time, to basically work with

7:00

him as a research assistant help him turn

7:02

these lectures into a book.

7:04

And that eventually became the book we

7:06

did also with Evan Roche, the

7:08

psychologist called the embodied mind,

7:10

that was published in ninety one, which is,

7:12

I think, fair to say, the first academic

7:15

book that explores the relationship between

7:18

Buddhist psychology and meditation

7:20

and cognitive science. And

7:22

pretty much ever since then, I've

7:24

been working to varying

7:26

degrees in that area, the book why

7:28

I'm not a Buddhist really came out of

7:31

much more recent work I had been doing in the

7:33

context of the Mind and Life Institute

7:35

and the dialogues that it has

7:38

fostered between scientists

7:40

and the Dalai Lama and

7:42

Buddhist scholars, especially in the Tibetan

7:44

tradition. it was through

7:46

my involvement in those

7:48

dialogues that I came to a place where I

7:50

felt I needed to write something that

7:52

expressed a more critical perspective on what I

7:54

saw going on in those dialogues, so

7:56

we can talk about more about that. But that's the

7:58

journey more or less from my

8:00

I both childhood really

8:02

into the writing of that book. So

8:04

the point that I want to emphasize

8:07

is that you've come to this with a

8:09

very deep background both in the

8:11

eastern thought and also

8:13

the cognitive science pieces

8:15

of this issue. I

8:17

doubt there's anybody else that

8:19

has quite the qualifications that you

8:21

do. There may be a few others,

8:23

but but yeah. Yeah.

8:30

I wanna take a moment to

8:32

talk about our longtime sponsor,

8:35

Text spander. Text

8:37

expander is great for anyone

8:39

who wants to reduce the time

8:41

wasted on repetitive typing.

8:43

You can avoid the need for copy and

8:45

paste by creating

8:47

snippets that expand into whatever

8:49

you need, including graphics

8:51

and links. Best of all, you

8:53

can sync your snippets across your

8:55

devices and even share them with

8:57

others. I literally

8:59

use text expander every

9:01

day and I really appreciate

9:03

that text expander is available

9:05

on Mac, Windows, Chrome,

9:08

iPhone, and iPad. brain

9:10

science listeners get twenty

9:12

percent off their first year.

9:14

Just visit texsixpander dot

9:17

com forward slash podcast

9:19

to learn more about text

9:21

expander. That's text

9:23

expander dot com forward

9:25

slash podcasts to learn

9:27

more and get twenty percent

9:29

off your first year.

9:35

Most of my listeners

9:37

have heard this claim that meditation

9:39

is a and

9:42

I just wanna get right to the

9:44

point. What is wrong with this

9:46

claim? So in a

9:48

nutshell, I would say that

9:50

it involves

9:52

a distortion of

9:55

what meditation is

9:58

to say that it's a science. If

9:59

by science, we mean the way we

10:02

understand science in the modern world

10:04

as an experimental method

10:06

with models and theories and

10:08

testing in an experimental context

10:10

with controls and and so on. So

10:13

meditation is a

10:15

practice in which

10:17

first of all, there's no such thing as just

10:19

meditation. Meditation is like the word

10:21

sports. There's, you know, there's

10:23

fencing, there's badminton, there's hockey,

10:25

there's all their soccer. Maybe chess is a

10:27

sport. The word is elastic and can

10:29

cover different kinds of things. So meditation

10:31

can cover many different

10:33

ways of training

10:36

or appointing trying to turn

10:38

this into a verb, making oneself acquainted

10:40

with the mind experientially or

10:43

contemplation this related

10:45

word. And when one does this,

10:47

one is usually in

10:49

a there are

10:51

secular of can talk about that. But usually

10:54

one is within a tradition that

10:56

provides a world view, religious

10:58

in nature, and one is given

11:00

various kinds of scripts and

11:02

instructions for

11:04

shaping the mind, for engaging

11:06

in a repeated practice where

11:08

one trains and shapes the mind

11:11

according to certain norms and ideals

11:13

and standards. So in that

11:15

way, it's more like art. It's a practice of self

11:18

the way, you know, learning to play the piano

11:20

or engaging in ballet, for example,

11:23

is. There are methods to it.

11:25

There is training. There is rigor.

11:27

But to say that it's a science in the sense

11:29

that you're just starting with some

11:31

kind of observation of the mind as

11:33

it's given to you, and then you're discovering

11:36

the nature of the mind is

11:38

incredibly simplistic because you're

11:40

bringing a whole set of

11:43

concepts and norms and

11:46

ideals into what you're doing. So to give a very

11:48

concrete illustration, I talk about this

11:50

in the book. I've done a number of these

11:52

kinds of retreats, but I went at one point

11:54

on a treat. There was a seven

11:56

day vipasana

11:58

or insight, Buddhist insight

11:59

meditation retreat. And it was

12:02

designed especially for scientists and

12:04

clinicians. This was in two

12:06

thousand and eight, I think. And

12:08

so many people who were involved in

12:10

the clinical use of mindfulness

12:12

and meditation practices or scientists

12:14

studying meditation. Many people

12:16

involved in the Mind of Life Institute. were

12:18

at this meditation retreat. And

12:21

the rhetoric that we were

12:23

given at the outset of the retreat,

12:25

which is a kind of typical modern

12:27

Western Buddhist rhetoric is you're

12:30

gonna drop all your assumptions

12:32

and concepts and preconceptions and

12:34

you are going to pay attention to the

12:37

mind as it is in the

12:39

present And you are

12:41

going to learn to introspect

12:44

precisely, see the mind

12:46

as it truly is. And

12:48

in doing that, you are engaging in

12:50

a kind of introspective science.

12:54

Now there is a sense in which part of

12:56

that is true, which is you you're

12:58

instructed to try to follow your breath as a

13:00

beginning practice and you're told,

13:02

you know, it's not about thinking. It's

13:04

about just keeping your mind on the

13:06

breath and then noticing what happens to

13:08

the mind. when you do that, it get

13:10

distracted. You feel certain things.

13:12

You have to return. You have to drop the

13:14

Meditation. Return your attention. you

13:17

come to feel how that's subtly modulated

13:19

by different thoughts and feeling states and

13:21

memories. So you do become experientially

13:25

enlivened and acquainted and attuned

13:27

with all sorts of things going on.

13:29

You're doing this eight to ten hours a day,

13:31

and so your metabolism slows

13:33

down. yet

13:35

what one is being told

13:37

that one's doing is practicing

13:39

a science of seeing things as they

13:41

really are. All along the way,

13:43

one is being given a kind of conceptual

13:45

framework and script for how to

13:47

understand and relate to the mind

13:49

that involves a kind of you

13:51

might say baby version or minimal

13:53

version of a Buddhist psychology, a

13:55

Buddhist taxonomy of mental states.

13:58

Ideas already that are

13:59

closely related to, say,

14:02

karma, to how the mind

14:04

moves and acts, the clinging

14:06

or desire that goes along with that.

14:08

the idea you should not identify with

14:10

that. So there's a kind of norm of disidentification.

14:13

And so that

14:15

is a skill. That is a practice.

14:19

But it's not a kind

14:21

of disinterested examination

14:23

of how things are

14:25

neutrally. It's a

14:27

guided scripted practice according to certain

14:29

values and norms of ethical

14:31

self cultivation. So

14:33

in saying that it's not a science. I don't mean to

14:35

say that it's anti science or opposed to

14:37

science. I mean to say it's just

14:39

different and that casting it in the

14:41

rhetoric of science really

14:44

falsifies what it is in the same

14:46

way as if you were to say that

14:48

sculpture or painting is a

14:50

science. It's a craft. It's a

14:52

skill. It takes expertise. admits

14:54

of different levels of accomplishment, but

14:57

it's not a science in

14:59

the sense of science that we valorize.

15:02

here and now in our world, which is, you know, modern experimental

15:05

science where we

15:07

are pursuing it not

15:09

according to specifically religious

15:12

values that have to do with a

15:14

worldview where transitoryness

15:17

is suffering and one shouldn't cling to it,

15:19

for example. to into

15:21

science as a norm, people immediately

15:23

say, no, that's that's not

15:25

appropriate. So that's what happens

15:27

when meditation gets cast as a

15:29

science as it gets stored in.

15:31

It arises also

15:33

from a particular

15:37

way of thinking

15:39

about science that I don't think is

15:41

particularly good for science, which is

15:43

that it instrumentalizes the

15:45

investigation. So it basically It

15:48

sees science as, like, a

15:50

technique of investigation

15:52

and mastery. Sort of, like, science is

15:54

about instrumental and

15:57

control. That is certainly part of

15:59

science, but science in its source is not

16:01

that. So it takes that conception

16:03

and it puts it onto the mind because if you

16:06

say something like as some meditation

16:08

teachers do that what you're doing when you're

16:10

practicing meditation is cultivating an

16:12

inner telescope with which you

16:14

can look at the mind, the way Galileo

16:16

turns his telescope onto the

16:18

planets, the minute you say something like that, you're

16:20

treating your own subjectivity, your

16:22

own mental life as a tool, as

16:24

an instrument. And when you do that,

16:26

you can't help but objectify

16:28

yourself inwardly. that's

16:31

distorting because subjectivity

16:33

or consciousness or lived

16:35

experience is precisely that which is

16:37

not objective in that sense or not

16:39

objectifiable. It can never be

16:41

fully rendered in that objectifiable

16:44

way. So it it takes a particularly

16:47

technological conception of science

16:49

and then grafts it onto introspection,

16:54

instrumentalizing the experience in

16:56

a way that I think actually distorts the

16:58

experience of what's happening. And it

17:00

also doesn't take into account

17:02

how

17:02

we've learned that so

17:04

much of what our brain is actually doing is

17:07

inaccessible

17:07

to introspection. And

17:09

as Robert Burton once said,

17:12

expecting the mind to tell you

17:14

how it works is talking to a con

17:16

man. But what about

17:18

the attitude that goes hand in hand

17:20

with this about identifying the

17:23

mind with the brain. Even

17:25

though that doesn't really seem necessarily

17:27

consistent with Buddhism, certainly

17:30

not certain forms of Buddhism. So

17:33

there, I would say that we have a way of

17:35

talking in the context of

17:37

meditation about the mind and the brain that I

17:39

think reflects kind of a

17:41

generally schizoid way we have of talking

17:43

about the mind and the brain and our culture at

17:45

large, which is On the one hand, a

17:47

discourse of meditation directly

17:49

changes the brain. It affects your brain. You

17:51

can train your brain.

17:54

on what level that's very shallow because anything you do affects

17:56

your brain. I mean, when I get up in the morning, the first thing I

17:58

do is, you know, have a pot of tea. I mean, that affects

18:00

my brain. My brain is

18:02

constantly being affected So, of

18:04

course, if I going to

18:06

affect my brain, the

18:08

idea that meditation

18:10

gets its value from training

18:12

the brain seems to be as

18:14

misguided as saying ballet gets its

18:16

value from training your

18:18

muscles. Of course, training your muscles

18:20

is part of ballet. But

18:23

the point of ballet lies in a

18:25

completely different domain, which is the domain of

18:27

art and aesthetics and the point of

18:30

meditation lies in a completely different domain, which is

18:32

either in a secular context, you

18:34

could say health and well-being, or in a

18:36

religious context, to say

18:38

in Asian religions, particularly Buddhism, it

18:40

has to do with a conception of

18:42

liberation and awakening and

18:44

enlightenment. and a view of the origins

18:46

and overcoming of suffering.

18:49

Casting the value of meditation in terms of

18:51

changing the brain, I think, is

18:53

misguided And then the

18:56

idea, as you were saying, that when

18:58

we practice meditation, we're

19:00

seeing the mind as it fundamentally is

19:03

that I would say is philosophically very

19:05

problematic for a number of reasons.

19:08

One, if we think of the mind

19:10

as really rooted

19:12

and intertwined with our whole

19:15

bodily life, including of

19:17

course, our brains, then that's

19:19

not directly disclosing

19:21

to consciousness. Consciousness is in a way

19:23

a skewed sample of what's going on in

19:25

terms of the larger dynamics of the

19:27

organism or the person done.

19:29

So the idea that

19:32

consciousness would be revealed or the

19:34

minds and its workings would be revealed in

19:36

meditation doesn't seem right. And

19:38

then moreover, Although some

19:40

philosophical traditions would disagree with this, I would say

19:42

that consciousness doesn't

19:44

reveal its own inner

19:46

nature and its own dependencies

19:49

on things other than

19:51

itself, it doesn't reveal that

19:53

from within. You can have

19:55

profound states of altered

19:58

consciousness or dissolution of the subject object

19:59

structure in which awareness has

20:04

a quality that is, let's

20:06

say, experientially invariant

20:08

across different changing

20:10

states in waking and dreaming. You know, I wrote

20:12

a lot about this in my book, waking, dreaming,

20:14

being, which about before. So, of course, you can

20:16

have states in which consciousness presents that

20:19

way, but it doesn't in presenting that

20:21

way, disclose it's

20:23

generative source if you wanna put it

20:25

that way. It's not

20:27

scruggable in that way. So to say

20:29

that, oh, I'm sitting down and the

20:31

nature of the mind in its inner nature is

20:33

going to be revealed to me from a false point

20:35

of view. That doesn't seem like a

20:37

legitimate statement.

20:37

You talked about something called a category

20:39

mistake. Would you expand on what that

20:42

means? So, I mean,

20:44

an example would be what I was saying

20:46

earlier where if we say

20:48

that the right

20:51

perspective from which to understand meditation

20:53

is the effects that it has on

20:55

the brain. that's a kind of category

20:57

mistake. It's it's a perfectly legitimate

20:59

scientific question to ask whether practice of

21:01

meditation in various settings

21:03

according to various sets.

21:06

has long term measurable changes

21:08

on the brain. There's nothing wrong with asking

21:10

that. However, it's a bit

21:12

like asking this is an example I use in

21:14

the book, whether the

21:16

long term practice of playing a musical

21:18

instrument has effects on the

21:20

brain, it stands to reason that

21:22

it does. that would be an

21:24

interesting thing to investigate if we're

21:26

interested in the effects of

21:28

music on the brain and the

21:30

place of musical cognition in

21:32

relation to other kinds of cognition. But to say then that

21:36

the meaning or value of

21:38

music, its significance in human life is

21:40

to be understood by looking

21:42

at brain systems that are affected by it. That's

21:44

a category of mistake. It's a confusion of levels.

21:46

It's like the example I use in my

21:48

book is, suppose we take Yoyomo.

21:52

and we record with great precision

21:55

using high density EEG or

21:57

we put Yoyama on an F

21:59

MRI scanner and we record

22:01

with spatial precision what's going

22:03

on in his brain

22:05

when he plays box

22:07

cello suite number one.

22:09

Well, of course, it stands to

22:12

reason that there may be some unique

22:14

neural signatures compared to

22:17

say, an amateur cello player or certainly

22:19

compared to someone who can't play the cello at

22:21

all. Of course, it stands. The reason they're gonna be

22:23

differences. But if we thought we could

22:25

understand music, let

22:27

alone bock simply

22:30

by looking at

22:32

the neural activity that would

22:34

be a kind of category mistake or confusion

22:36

of levels. So, similarly, the

22:38

idea that we can establish

22:40

the value or validity of meditation

22:43

or understand what it is and what its purpose is by

22:45

looking at neural systems

22:47

affected by it. That's just the same

22:49

confusion. I was thinking we can understand bock

22:51

through looking at happens in your your mom's brain

22:53

when he's in the scanner. In other words,

22:55

meditation is a it's an individual

22:57

practice, but it's also a social practice,

22:59

and it's one that takes place in

23:01

a culture. So involves communities

23:03

of practitioners, culturally

23:05

transmitted norms, and conceptual frameworks, and you're

23:07

not gonna understand any of that if you're

23:09

looking just at the brain. Now, of

23:11

course, you know, neuroscientists who

23:14

work on meditation know this. I

23:16

mean, it's not that neuroscientists

23:18

are unaware of this. and neuroscientists who

23:20

are doing some of the most

23:22

pioneering and interesting work are very

23:24

concerned to bring, say, anthropologists

23:28

into the collaborative effort

23:30

to get a richer understanding

23:32

of social context. Cognitive scientists

23:35

in general who are doing the best work on meditation, they're aware of

23:37

this. But this is not reflected often

23:39

in the hype, even sometimes their own

23:41

hype. And the general

23:43

larger cultural discourse around meditation in the brain.

23:49

Now a

23:51

word from our sponsor better

23:53

help. On brain science, we

23:55

often explore ways that we can

23:57

keep our brains healthy such

23:59

as exercise

23:59

and learning new things. But

24:02

sometimes we can't

24:03

go it alone. That's

24:05

where better help online

24:07

therapy comes in. Better help

24:09

is convenient, accessible, affordable,

24:12

and

24:12

entirely online. All you have

24:14

to do to be matched with a therapist is to fill

24:16

out a brief survey and

24:19

you can switch therapists at any time.

24:21

Although I haven't used

24:25

online therapy, Over the years, I

24:27

have found working with a therapist

24:29

helpful at various times.

24:31

So I

24:31

feel good about

24:33

sharing better help. with

24:35

you. When you

24:36

want to be a better problem

24:39

solver, therapy can get you

24:41

there. Visit better help dot

24:43

com slash ginger today to get

24:45

ten percent off your first

24:47

month. That's better HELP

24:50

dot com slash ginger.

24:52

Before

24:54

we talk

24:58

about the embodied approach, can you

25:00

talk a little bit more about

25:02

just this whole conflating of

25:04

the mind and brain. because

25:07

I think that's really an important

25:10

idea. So the way that I think

25:12

about this is if you

25:14

say the mind is in the brain

25:16

or the mind is what

25:18

the brain does, it's rather like

25:21

saying that flight is in the wings

25:23

of a bird or that flight is

25:25

what the wings of the bird does.

25:27

you can't have flight without wings and you

25:29

can't have a human mind without a

25:31

human brain. But

25:35

the wings generate lift,

25:37

which enables flight, and flight is

25:39

an activity of the whole animal in

25:41

its environment. So I think

25:44

of mind as an activity of the

25:46

whole organism or animal

25:48

or person in its environment. And

25:50

it comes about through the way that the brain

25:52

is able to relate us

25:55

to each other and to the environment, that's

25:58

where the domain of

25:59

mind or mental phenomena resides

26:03

not inside the

26:05

brain. Another example would be

26:07

to say that for example,

26:09

a cathedral isn't in its stones. You

26:11

need the stones for the cathedral,

26:13

but the cathedral as

26:15

a cathedral exists in

26:17

a public social sphere and

26:20

its architecture is determined

26:22

by conventions

26:24

and history and iconography

26:27

and stories about the gospels and so

26:29

on. And so the cathedral exists in

26:31

that domain of meaning

26:33

and the stones just considered

26:35

as stones they don't exist in that

26:37

domain of meaning unless you see

26:39

them through the cathedral. So if you

26:41

see the brain through the mind, in other words, you

26:43

see the brain, as

26:45

an organ or system in the

26:47

organism embedded in its environment that

26:49

is enabling the repertoire of

26:51

cognitive phenomena that we see different animals

26:55

capable of, then you see the

26:57

brain in light of the mind. But if you just say

26:59

the mind is in the brain, then

27:01

you miss that. So that in a

27:03

nutshell is sort of an analogy for

27:05

the embodied embedded view of

27:07

cognition. Howard Bauchner: And so this

27:09

embodied approach doesn't just apply to

27:11

people. Right? Any animal

27:13

with a certain amount of brainpower

27:15

has a mind that

27:18

consists of being embodied and

27:20

interacting with the world. Yeah.

27:22

I think so. So in animal

27:24

life, we know that the

27:26

nervous system is the crucial player for this.

27:28

The nervous system coupled to the rest of the body,

27:30

of course. There is an interesting question

27:33

whether we want to talk about mind or

27:36

cognition in a larger sense that

27:38

encompasses all of biological life.

27:40

I mean, if you see a bacterium able

27:43

to differentiate upwards of forty

27:45

different chemical gradients and

27:47

swim towards and away things and

27:49

do so where it's able to

27:52

keep track of the changes

27:54

in the rate of concentration of the

27:56

molecules over time and behave

27:58

appropriately. And so it has a kind of

27:59

version of bacterial memory, do we

28:02

wanna call that mind or cognition? I

28:04

mean, in a very general sense, I would

28:06

say, yes, it's not animal cognition,

28:08

but it's I sometimes use the

28:10

word which comes from Francis Corvella, the word

28:12

sense making that the organism makes

28:14

sense of its environment and that if you

28:16

don't wanna say that's mind, you can

28:18

say it's the roots of mind. It's

28:20

the precursor of mind. And

28:22

by some definitions, it's definitely

28:24

cognition. If you think of cognition

28:27

as being something that involves

28:29

some kind of decision.

28:31

So if you're gonna go toward or

28:34

away from a not just chemical, to

28:36

me that qualifies. What about the

28:40

claim that a

28:42

neuroscience has confirmed

28:44

that the self does not

28:47

exist. Yes. So this is one

28:49

that we hear a lot. Philosophers say

28:51

this and it gets repeated in

28:53

popular writings. So if we

28:55

think of the self in a very narrow way, we think

28:57

of it as a

28:59

unified Ginger

29:02

unchanged subject that

29:04

would also be the source of

29:07

agency. So an interchanging subject

29:09

and agent of action we think that in

29:11

that very narrow way, then

29:13

we could say, well, our

29:15

best science of the mind

29:18

doesn't give us any reason to believe that

29:20

the self is like that or that there is that

29:22

kind of a self. Because the

29:24

self or the person is something

29:26

that emerges through the

29:28

interaction of the brain body

29:30

cognition, environment, social

29:33

system, norms, So the

29:35

self, I would say, is a kind

29:37

of construction. So when people

29:39

say the self is an illusion, they

29:41

often think of

29:43

the self in this narrow way. And

29:45

then they think, we can't find any self like

29:47

that in the brain. So

29:49

then, therefore, there is no self but

29:51

it sure feels like there is that kind

29:53

of self, so it must be an illusion.

29:55

So I would say, first of

29:57

all, that that's too narrow a conception

30:00

of self. that the self is a construction, not

30:02

an illusion, not everything that's a

30:04

construction is illusory, and

30:07

that looking for it inside the brain

30:09

is not the right place to look for it. That's again, like,

30:11

trying to find flight inside the wings of

30:13

the bird. And that

30:16

this idea of

30:19

feeling that there is this kind of, like, inter

30:21

essential me behind my eyes that

30:23

also was, like, the source of my decisions

30:26

it's true that sometimes we feel like there's a

30:28

self like that. But I don't think

30:30

that's normally how we feel there is

30:32

a self. I think normally we feel

30:35

our selfhood in an

30:38

engaged way geared into the

30:40

world interaction with others

30:42

and that we don't feel it as this kind of

30:44

like distant spectator. In

30:46

fact, feeling it is that kind of distant spectator

30:48

is often a symptom of a kind

30:50

of depersonalization. So the whole language of

30:52

the self is an illusion. It's

30:54

a tendentious language because it starts

30:56

with a very narrow assumption

30:59

about how we should think

31:01

of what a self is or what a self

31:03

could be. And then it

31:05

superimposes it onto the

31:07

brain And then, of course, you're gonna come

31:09

up empty handed if you cast it that way.

31:11

But then, why would we cast it

31:13

that way from the beginning? I mean, there are

31:15

historical reasons why in

31:18

some context, it gets cast that way.

31:20

But generally speaking, I think we can

31:22

see with a little bit of reflection that's not

31:24

the right way to cast things. I think

31:26

we've got time to talk about one other

31:28

issue that's in there that relates to

31:30

actual I mean, you

31:32

have some wonderful chapters in

31:34

there about buddhist modernism, which we aren't gonna get

31:36

into, even though I'm gonna

31:38

encourage my readers to read the book,

31:40

to learn what that is because it's

31:42

really fascinating. There was

31:44

one chapter about a writer

31:46

who's written a book, why Buddhism

31:48

is True. It's really just

31:50

basically based on evolutionary

31:52

psychology, which that's not

31:54

even mainstream Neuroscience. But

31:57

Would you just talk a little

31:59

bit

31:59

about that argument? because that's

32:02

another thing people will hear. Buddhism

32:04

is somehow magically more scientific

32:06

and is because it's supported by Neuroscience.

32:09

This is

32:09

Robert Wright's book, why Buddhism is true. And

32:11

basically, what Wright does in that book, and I

32:13

should say for anybody who's interested

32:15

right invited me on to his podcast, and we had

32:17

a really long, I don't know, hour and a

32:20

half to hour conversation about his

32:22

book, my book, my critique, and, you

32:24

know, it was a great conversation. He's a

32:26

very generous person. So people can

32:28

check out if out if they want it on YouTube.

32:30

What's the name of his podcast? His podcast,

32:32

oh, I'm forgetting the name of it.

32:34

But if you it's Robert Wright with

32:36

a, yeah, WRIGHT

32:39

I'll look it up and put it in the show notes. Yeah. It's in

32:41

YouTube, the conversation that we hey. You actually interviewed me

32:43

twice. One for waking and dreaming being and one for

32:45

why I'm not able to stand. So they're

32:47

both in YouTube. So he wrote a book

32:49

called why Buddhism is Drew. And he

32:52

basically argued that if we strip

32:54

away from Buddhism, it's

32:56

let's say, religious metaphysical elements, and

32:58

we focus on its

33:02

psychological aspects that we

33:04

can make consistent with

33:06

or compatible with modern

33:08

science, that those

33:10

are true according

33:12

to the framework of evolutionary psychology.

33:14

So he uses evolutionary psychology,

33:17

which is the idea

33:19

that our mines

33:21

and brains were shaped

33:23

in the pleistocene under certain

33:25

selective pressures so that we have this kind

33:27

of collection of cognitive modules that have

33:29

been selected for various functions,

33:32

and we are according to

33:34

the evolutionary psychologist stone

33:37

age minds in the modern world

33:40

and says that

33:42

conception of the mind fits with the Buddhist

33:44

diagnosis of why we suffer

33:46

and why we experience craving and

33:49

attachment. And Buddhism gives us a way

33:51

of dealing with that predicament. So

33:54

I have a critique of this that involves

33:56

a number of different things.

33:58

One is the way that he, you

34:00

might say, sanitizes

34:03

Buddhism by trying to render it in a

34:05

particularly naturalistic way by the likes of

34:07

evolutionary psychology and how that distorts

34:11

core Buddhist commitments to

34:14

ideas of awakening and

34:16

ideas about the sort of ephemeral

34:18

or impermanent nature of the world. And

34:20

then I have a critique of evolutionary psychology, which is I just don't

34:22

think that the evolutionary psychology model of the

34:25

mind is credible on scientific grounds.

34:27

I don't think that the

34:29

brain is evolutionary psychology ways of understanding

34:31

what a module is. I think

34:33

that the trajectory of neuroscience

34:35

in the past twenty

34:37

five, thirty years has been to show the kind of

34:40

multiplexing and plasticity of

34:42

brain systems that make them not

34:44

identifiable with discrete

34:46

functional modules. I think

34:48

the Darwinian selectionist way

34:50

of understanding evolution that underwrites

34:52

evolutionary psychology is also problematic

34:54

within the domain of evolutionary biology.

34:57

So we could go into that, but that would be sort

34:59

of a whole other thing. And I describe

35:01

my reasons for on strictly

35:03

scientific grounds, not being persuaded by evolution

35:05

a nice psychology and then how that sort of

35:07

undercuts the argument for a

35:10

naturalized modern version of Buddhism being

35:12

true in his words. the appropriate question

35:14

is about Buddhism isn't really whether it's

35:16

true under that rendering.

35:18

The appropriate question is whether it's

35:20

a valuable human tradition with insight

35:22

that we can learn from in conversation with other traditions. And

35:24

that leads into the discussion of

35:26

of cosmopolitanism in the latter part of

35:28

the book or the end last chapter of

35:32

the book. Now,

35:34

I

35:36

want to take a moment

35:36

to tell you about our newest

35:39

sponsor, masterclass. with

35:42

master class you can learn from the best

35:45

minds anytime, anywhere and at

35:47

your own pace. You can learn

35:50

how to act from Helen

35:52

Marin. Improve your scientific thinking and

35:54

communication skills

35:56

with NeoDegress Tyson or learn creative writing with

35:58

Margaret Atwood. With

35:59

over a hundred and eighty classes from

36:02

a range of world

36:04

class instructors that thing

36:06

you've always wanted to do is

36:08

closer than you think.

36:10

One of the things I love about

36:13

master class is the content is broken down

36:15

into small chunks that you can fit

36:17

into your busy life. So

36:20

far, my favorite

36:22

class is US presidential history and leadership

36:24

with Doris Kern's good

36:26

one. For example, she talks

36:28

about the

36:30

importance of empathy because a truly great

36:32

leader wants to

36:32

make things better for

36:35

other people. She talks

36:37

about the role of

36:40

hardship and how persefering through

36:42

hardship not only gives

36:44

one confidence for the future, but

36:46

also helps one

36:48

learn empathy. To me, this class is nothing

36:50

short of inspirational. I

36:52

highly recommend you check

36:54

it out. This

36:56

holiday, give one annual

36:58

membership and get one free.

37:00

Go to masterclass dot com

37:02

forward slash

37:04

ginger today. That's masterclass dot com forward

37:06

slash ginger

37:08

terms apply.

37:13

I want to talk about

37:16

cosmopolitanism, but first, I want to

37:18

make a comment about Wright's

37:20

embrace of

37:22

evolutionary psychology. I understand

37:24

that his background

37:26

was, but the middle was a baptismal,

37:28

of some sort. So I was struck

37:30

by the fact that he has

37:32

basically replaced original sin

37:34

with now we're stuck with the

37:36

caveman brain. Okay. It seems like

37:39

the same problem. We've just We're

37:41

still horribly flawed in that view. Yeah. I think there is an element

37:43

of that in his narrative, which is

37:45

this Darwinian evolutionary psychology

37:48

rendering of

37:50

our fundamental flaw that would

37:52

be original sin in the religious

37:54

narrative. And completely discounting

37:56

the fact that our success really

37:59

comes from being flexible and

38:02

adaptable. That just really

38:03

struck me really strongly. What

38:05

else would you like to share

38:07

before we close? Maybe I'll

38:09

just

38:09

say a little bit about work that I'm

38:12

doing now to wet people's appetite.

38:14

So I just finished a book with

38:16

two physicists our fellow

38:18

Glyser, who's a theoretical physicist at

38:20

Dartmouth and Adam Frank, who's an

38:22

astrophysicist at University of Rochester.

38:24

The book is called The Blind

38:26

Spot Experience science and the search And

38:28

it's a big picture, big idea book

38:30

that looks at the

38:34

relationship between science and lived

38:36

experience and the puzzles and

38:38

conundrums that science gets

38:40

itself into when it forgets its

38:43

source in lived experience. And so

38:45

we chart that across cosmology and

38:47

the nature of time and quantum physics,

38:49

the nature of matter, and neuroscience, and the

38:51

nature of consciousness, biology and the

38:53

nature of life. We end with a discussion of the planet and the

38:55

climate crisis. So that book

38:58

will probably be out

39:00

in ice

39:02

Spectrum, it will be out in early twenty twenty four, maybe late

39:04

twenty twenty three. MIT press is going to

39:06

publish it. I'm not sure the exact timetable. We're

39:09

just doing the sort

39:11

of final production revision of it now. So I

39:13

wanna mention that. And then a book that I'm

39:16

really in the early stages

39:18

of writing I can't say exactly

39:20

when it's going to appear is about

39:22

dying, the experience of

39:24

dying, and

39:27

the insight or light that can be shed on

39:29

that through contemplative practices

39:34

for meeting death and

39:36

contemplative practices in spice for

39:38

helping people undergo dying

39:41

combined with new

39:44

emerging research in neuroscience about what

39:46

happens to the brain in the dying

39:48

process. And that book I say

39:50

is probably still a couple

39:52

years away. And my

39:55

traditional question, advice

39:57

for students. You're further along

39:59

in your

39:59

career now. and change

40:02

universities. So I'd

40:04

like to have your current take on that

40:06

question. I think it

40:08

depends on the field my

40:11

feel is philosophy. I think one thing that's interesting,

40:13

at least in my experience, that's happening in

40:15

philosophy that I really want to encourage, and I think this

40:17

has to do with the

40:20

pandemic. you know, I have about

40:22

ten PhD students. They're all working on really significant

40:24

fundamental topics that affect

40:26

people's lives, things like grief,

40:29

and dying anxiety and they're using

40:32

neuroscience and phenomenology and philosophy

40:34

of mind to cast light on

40:36

these things. So I think

40:38

that is something I would encourage in

40:40

students that if you have an interest in something

40:42

that really has to do with sort of the existential

40:44

predicament of being human, you

40:46

should follow through with that and use that to enlivant

40:48

philosophy and use philosophy to eliminate

40:50

it. In

40:52

cognitive science, I think this

40:54

is an amazing time for

40:56

advances in collaborative

40:58

activity among neuroscientists and

41:01

psychologists. and anthropologists. And I think the one advice

41:03

there I would give, especially to undergraduates

41:05

and also to graduate

41:08

students, is really to try to work

41:10

in a transdisciplinary

41:12

way that links these different fields. I

41:14

mean, of course, if you're a molecular neurobiologist,

41:18

that's you know, you've got to be honest to the field of molecular or

41:20

neurobiology. If you're a

41:22

cognitive neuroscientist, you have to be honest

41:24

to the techniques of

41:26

EEG and F MRI and all that. But it's very

41:28

important, I think, to step

41:30

back and have a bigger

41:32

picture and a sense of

41:34

connection to other scientists,

41:36

particularly if you're studying the mind, those

41:38

who work in disciplines like Anthropologie, I

41:41

think the social context of

41:43

the mind is really important. So putting the

41:45

cognitive and neuroscience perspective together with the

41:47

say cognitive anthropology perspective, I think there's a

41:49

really great move that neuroscientists are working on, but it's still a

41:51

small group of people. So that's a direction I would

41:53

certainly encourage. So you closed the

41:55

book, Evan, with

41:58

talking about cosmopolitanism. And

41:59

I would like you to

42:02

tell us about

42:04

that. Yes.

42:05

So cosmopolitanism is

42:07

a philosophical idea that in

42:09

the western traditional philosophy goes

42:11

back to the Greeks. and

42:13

to the idea that we are all one

42:15

human family and we should

42:18

identify as citizens of the

42:20

world or as members of the human community

42:22

first and foremost rather than

42:24

exclusively with our immediate

42:26

local group or local tradition. And

42:28

in the context of the book, I

42:31

make the point that this way of thinking of there

42:33

being multiple human traditions that

42:36

don't have to agree with each other, but that can

42:38

engage in conversation with each other

42:40

about ethics, about

42:42

transformation, about mental cultivation,

42:44

that this is really the

42:46

context within which we should see

42:48

science Buddhism dialogue or the science meditation conversation Meditation

42:52

than a context in which

42:54

one tries to use

42:56

science to say prove

42:58

Buddhism or use Buddhism to

43:00

embellish Neuroscience or

43:02

Neuroscience to

43:04

prove the value of meditation or use

43:07

meditation to enhance science that

43:09

we should see this

43:11

in the larger space of a

43:14

conversation between different traditions

43:16

where attrition to be

43:19

specific like buddhism is a rich deep tradition with many insights

43:21

into the mind coming from a rigorous

43:24

intellectual tradition that's philosophical and

43:26

psychological. And of course, we can learn

43:28

from that But the way

43:30

to engage it is not

43:32

to go in and try

43:34

to prove it in a sort of partisan

43:36

way, nor is it to use

43:38

it to embellish

43:40

concepts about brain function

43:43

or concepts about whether we

43:45

are fundamentally pro social animals

43:47

or things like that. So that's kind

43:49

of how caused politics functions for me in the book. I I'm

43:52

gonna include some of those references in

43:54

my show

43:56

notes because I've really enjoyed reading about that. And I think

43:58

it really has a lot to say for

44:00

our current situation

44:02

that to find what we have

44:04

in common and appreciate

44:07

our differences is a balancing

44:09

act and it's got to have

44:11

both parts. Well,

44:12

I really enjoyed your book and I

44:13

look forward to getting the new one, the

44:16

blind spot. I usually get

44:18

MIT press

44:20

book So I will definitely look forward to that.

44:22

And I'm glad I found this

44:24

one because I'll just say in

44:28

closing that when I read your book, why I'm not a Buddhist, I I

44:30

had all these discomforts about

44:32

why I didn't feel

44:34

like Buddhist

44:36

really was quite the right like what you said, why

44:38

you're not a bit as it really spoke

44:40

to me personally, you know. But the other

44:43

thing you said that I wanna mention is that you point out

44:45

in your book that there's no one

44:47

Buddhism. That Buddhism really is I mean,

44:49

it's just like

44:52

Christianity or even Islam. It's got many different

44:54

schools of thought, so to speak.

44:56

And a long tradition of debating, and

44:58

I think it's really important for those

45:02

outside the tradition to realize that even though

45:04

the insight meditation people claim, hey,

45:06

we're getting back to the basics. what

45:10

everybody always does? Yeah.

45:12

Yeah. That's a a claim that many

45:14

people make. But if you look at it from a

45:16

historian's point of view, it's not accurate. put

45:19

it as many things that's always been evolving. And and

45:21

that doesn't invalidate the way that people pursue

45:23

it here and now. But the claim that

45:25

in pursuing it here and now, they're sort of recovering

45:27

an original form that should

45:29

be viewed with a great deal of

45:32

suspicion. I read history of Buddhism a while

45:34

back. I don't think I read the particular book

45:36

that you referred to

45:38

in this book, but I was struck by the fact that from its very

45:40

beginning, Buddhism has always adapted to

45:42

the culture it moved to. I mean, you

45:44

take going from India

45:46

to China, Evan at that

45:47

stage. It changed to

45:49

fit China. It went to

45:51

Japan. It changed to fit Japan. It came

45:53

to the west. It changed to

45:55

fit the west. you know, as

45:57

kind of opposite from Christianity goes and tries to make the people wherever it

45:59

goes, changed to

46:02

fit in. Yeah.

46:04

I think that's mainly a political thing difference between

46:06

them. They're both missionary religion, so they

46:08

both see converts. Which is something I

46:10

think people don't always appreciate. I

46:13

know that's a little off the subject

46:15

of neuroscience. But since there's so

46:17

many claims about biz

46:20

amount in the brain, I think it's worth

46:22

talking about. Thanks again for taking the time to talk with me.

46:24

Yeah. Thanks for inviting me again, and I'm

46:26

glad you discovered this book since it

46:28

doesn't show up on the neuroscience list, of

46:30

course, not being

46:32

a neuroscience but I'm glad you

46:34

found it and then it spoke to

46:36

you. Hope that means a lot

46:38

to me. Before I

46:39

review the key ideas, I

46:42

today's interview, I want to share a few brief announcements.

46:44

First, I want to thank those

46:46

of you who support Brain Science financially.

46:50

This podcast is independently produced and depends

46:52

on the support of listeners like

46:54

you. There are several ways that

46:56

you can support my work including

46:59

MyLipson Premium Subscription, Patreon and

47:01

Single Donations via

47:04

Paypal.

47:06

please visit brain science podcast dot comdonations for

47:10

a detailed description of these

47:14

options. so that you can choose the one that fits both your needs

47:16

and your budget. However,

47:18

even if you can't support brain science financially,

47:22

You can help by sharing the show with others. Thanks

47:24

again for your support. Next,

47:26

I wanna remind you that

47:28

my book, are you sure, the

47:32

unconscious origins of certainty is

47:34

available in both ebook and

47:36

paper back from all the

47:38

major online book sellers. Please

47:40

consider gifting this book to someone you care

47:42

about. And if you're interested

47:44

in an autograph copy, just

47:46

email me

47:48

at brainscience podcast at gmail dot com. Finally, don't

47:50

forget to email me if you're

47:52

interested in helping with

47:54

or attending a talk or

47:56

meet up in April twenty

47:58

twenty three, either in

47:59

Amsterdam or Zurich. I

48:02

will be taking a cruise down the line

48:05

and I would like to use this as a chance

48:07

to meet listeners who live along

48:09

the route. Now I want to start

48:11

my episode review by thanking Evan

48:14

Thompson for taking the time to talk

48:16

with me again. I also want

48:18

to talk briefly about

48:20

why I chose this topic.

48:22

First, as an independent podcaster, I have

48:24

the freedom

48:25

to choose topics that I

48:27

find personally compelling. That

48:31

is certainly the case for the claim that meditation

48:33

is a mind science.

48:35

Before I launch

48:38

brain science, I considered myself a Buddhist. But

48:40

as I began to learn more about

48:42

how the brain really works, I

48:45

could not harmonize the dualistic Buddhist notion

48:47

of the mind with what I

48:49

was learning. More

48:52

importantly, Many Western buddhists and even respected

48:54

scientists have accepted the

48:57

claim that meditation is

48:59

a mind science. This

49:02

underpins the much larger

49:04

claim that Buddhism is

49:08

uniquely scientific. So

49:09

let's

49:10

review the problems

49:12

with this claim. Thompson

49:15

says that his objections are both

49:17

scientific and philosophic. Though it's fair to say that the focus

49:19

of today's conversation was on his

49:22

philosophical objections.

49:26

Why

49:26

does he reject the claim that

49:28

meditation is an introspective

49:30

science? His

49:32

main objection is that meditation

49:35

does not, as is often

49:37

claimed, provide a pure

49:39

neutral account of what is happening in

49:41

the mind. There are several

49:43

reasons for this. Whatever

49:46

sort of meditation one practices,

49:48

one has given instructions and

49:50

expectations that inevitably influence

49:54

the experience. A more obvious objection is that

49:56

the experience is not

49:58

independently verifiable.

49:59

Thompson didn't mention

50:02

this, but does

50:04

disqualify Meditation as a

50:06

scientific practice. Galileo

50:08

is often held up as a

50:10

role model of the scientific method

50:13

because the observations he did

50:15

with his telescope could be

50:17

repeated or replicated by

50:20

others. This is not possible

50:22

with meditation. Although, this

50:24

didn't come up during

50:27

our conversation, another

50:30

huge issue is that neuroscientists have learned that

50:32

most of what the brain does is

50:34

inaccessible to the

50:36

conscious mind. Thus even

50:38

if we could get objective

50:40

information through introspection, it

50:42

would be

50:44

woefully incomplete. But perhaps

50:46

the biggest argument in favor

50:48

of meditation seems to be that it

50:50

changes the brain in

50:52

ways that

50:54

can supposedly be replicated in different

50:56

meditators. Is that a

50:58

valid

50:58

argument in its favor?

51:01

Thompson says no because

51:04

everything you do changes your

51:06

brain. Actually, I

51:07

think that brings us to a much

51:09

more important problem with claiming

51:12

meditation is a mind

51:14

science. Because the

51:14

people making these claims are

51:16

often enamored of brain imaging, They

51:19

also tend to conflate the mind and the brain. The

51:21

whole point of embodied

51:23

cognition, which actually is

51:25

a philosophical position, is

51:28

that the mind is more than the brain, saying that

51:31

the mind is embodied recognizes

51:33

that the brain is

51:35

embedded in a body within

51:37

a world. In

51:40

his book, why I am non Thompson

51:43

introduced an analogy that he also

51:45

shared in the interview. He said,

51:47

birds need wings to fly, but

51:49

flight is not in the

51:51

wings. Sometimes those with

51:53

a brain centric view

51:56

will say, that the mind is what the brain

51:58

does. But

51:58

that's like saying

51:59

flying is what

52:02

wings do. but it

52:03

takes the entire bird to

52:06

fly. That's why

52:07

he said, mind is

52:09

an activity of the

52:12

whole organism. We

52:14

touched briefly on several other claims that

52:16

tend to go along with the brain

52:18

focused approach to meditation One

52:20

was the idea that neuroscience has proven the Buddhist claim that

52:23

the self does not exist.

52:27

here Here, The

52:28

idea is realizing that there is

52:30

no permanent unchanging self does

52:32

not make the self that the

52:36

brain constructs unreal or

52:38

an illusion. Modern physics has

52:40

taught us that solid objects are mostly

52:42

empty, but that doesn't make

52:45

the objects an illusion or

52:47

unreal. The fact that our

52:49

conscious experience of our inner world

52:51

is unreliable does not

52:53

negate the experience but

52:55

we can't look to it for an objective

52:58

understanding of what the mind is

53:00

doing. We also talked

53:02

about Robert Wright's use of evolutionary

53:04

psychology to prove that

53:07

Buddhism is true. I was struck by

53:09

the similarity of the claim that we are

53:11

stuck in a caveman brain

53:14

and original sin, but the strongest objections to

53:17

evolutionary psychology, rests on its

53:19

dependence on a modular view

53:21

of the mind that

53:23

does not fit contemporary

53:26

neuroscience. This chapter is

53:28

great if you're looking for a concise

53:30

critique of

53:32

evolutionary psychology. At the

53:34

end of the interview, we

53:36

talked very briefly about cosmopolitanism.

53:38

This is the idea that

53:40

we are citizens of the world

53:42

but that doesn't mean that we all have to have beliefs or

53:45

ways of life.

53:47

Cosmopolitanism offers

53:49

a ray of hope in our divided

53:52

world. I will include several

53:54

references about it in the

53:56

show notes. Much of Evan

53:57

Thompson's book, Wyam Evan, is

53:59

an argument against the claim

54:01

of many West

54:04

turn Buddhist, that Buddhism is uniquely scientific and

54:06

maybe not even a religion. He shows

54:08

why neither of

54:09

these claims stand up

54:11

to either scientific or

54:14

historical scrutiny. Obviously,

54:16

that topic is beyond the

54:18

scope of this podcast, but I

54:21

think Evan Thompson's book why I

54:23

am not a Buddhist is a worthwhile read for anyone who wants to learn

54:25

more about these topics.

54:28

That being said, I want to

54:29

close with a couple

54:32

of themes that have been running through brain science for many

54:36

years. First, since most of

54:38

our brain does

54:40

is not accessible introspection.

54:42

We cannot use meditation

54:44

or any other introspective technique,

54:48

including psychoanalysis, to determine

54:50

how it works. Even more

54:52

importantly, the mind is embodied.

54:55

This means that it's embedded

54:57

in a body that interacts

54:59

with its environment, you are not

55:01

a brain in a

55:04

bat. And because the

55:06

brain is not the mind, brain

55:08

alone is not enough.

55:11

I make

55:12

this last point because even though

55:15

I create a show about the brain.

55:17

I've tried not to fall prey

55:19

to neuromania. We are also

55:21

wired to be social. So

55:23

being human means being parts of

55:26

cultures. These cultures are

55:28

also constantly changing our

55:30

brains as well as our minds.

55:33

I

55:33

would love to

55:34

hear your feedback about this episode. Feel

55:36

free to send me email at

55:39

Neuroscience podcast at gmail dot

55:41

com. And can find complete show notes and

55:44

episode transcripts on my

55:46

website at brainscience Podcast

55:48

dot com. You can get

55:50

shown us automatically every month.

55:52

If you sign up for the free newsletter,

55:54

just go to Neuroscience dot

55:57

com or text brain science, all one word,

55:59

to

55:59

55444

56:02

When you sign up, you'll get a free

56:04

gift entitled Michael things

56:06

you need to know about your brain. That's

56:08

text. Brain signs all one

56:10

word to 55444

56:14

Next month, I will be sharing the sixteenth

56:16

Annual Review episode. Until then,

56:18

I hope you will

56:19

check out my

56:22

other podcast

56:22

grain rainbows and books and ideas. Thanks again for listening. I

56:24

look forward to talking with you again

56:28

very soon.

56:31

Brain science is

56:33

copyrighted by Virginia Campbell MD. You

56:35

may share the show with others, but

56:37

for any other uses

56:40

or derivatives, please write

56:42

to brain science podcast at gmail

56:44

dot com for permission.

56:46

The theme

56:46

music for brain science is

56:50

written and performed by Tony Cutruccia. You can

56:52

find his work at syncopationnow

56:56

dot com.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features