Podchaser Logo
Home
How to Stop Obsessing Over “What If…?”

How to Stop Obsessing Over “What If…?”

Released Thursday, 27th October 2022
 1 person rated this episode
How to Stop Obsessing Over “What If…?”

How to Stop Obsessing Over “What If…?”

How to Stop Obsessing Over “What If…?”

How to Stop Obsessing Over “What If…?”

Thursday, 27th October 2022
 1 person rated this episode
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

How

0:00

do you find opportunities in

0:02

hard situations? And are you ready to

0:04

reach your wouldn't go back moment? That's

0:07

what I help you do in my new book, which

0:09

just like this podcast is called Build

0:11

For Tomorrow. It's a guide to help

0:13

anyone who's going through big change in their

0:15

work or life. and is full of

0:18

exercises, lessons, and big

0:20

concepts you need to know, like how

0:22

to work your next job and how to change

0:24

before you must. along with stories

0:26

from the smartest people in business and

0:28

the history of innovation. Stuff

0:30

that frankly I learned while making this

0:32

podcast and then I expanded to figure out

0:35

how it can help you. Because this book

0:37

is designed to help you thrive. Reinvention,

0:40

it's not about grit. It's a process anyone

0:42

can learn and since this book has come out, I've

0:44

heard from so many people who said it helped

0:46

them figure out what they really want and

0:48

then go get it. Hild for tomorrow

0:50

is available in hardcover, audio

0:53

book, and e book, and you can find it

0:55

wherever you find books, whether that's

0:57

Amazon, Barnes and Noble, your local retailer,

0:59

or jason pfeiffer dot com slash

1:02

book. Again, my book is called

1:04

Build For Tomorrow.

1:07

This is Build For Tomorrow, a podcast

1:09

about the smartest solutions to our most

1:11

misunderstood stood problems. I'm Jason

1:14

Feifer. Have you ever done

1:16

something and it did not go

1:18

as you hoped and then you just

1:20

felt empty. And

1:22

you started obsessing over it,

1:24

like going through every detail over

1:27

and over. Imagine all the ways

1:29

you could have done better Well, I

1:31

recently had a kind of

1:33

embarrassing experience like this and it made

1:36

me feel a little crazy and then it sent me

1:38

down a rabbit hole to understand what

1:40

was happening to me? And I wanna

1:42

tell you about it because what I found was

1:44

pretty helpful. And maybe the next

1:46

time you are obsessing over what could

1:48

have been, It can be helpful to you

1:50

too. So some context.

1:53

As you may know, I released a book in

1:55

September. It's called Build For Tomorrow, just

1:57

like this pod and as part of promoting

1:59

the book, I asked every

2:02

big podcaster I know if they would

2:04

have me on their show because, you know, podcasts

2:06

are one of the best ways to promote books. And

2:08

for the most part, everyone said yes, which was

2:11

awesome. And then the tapings all went

2:13

pretty well, which was also awesome except

2:17

for two of them. Those

2:19

did not go so well. And when

2:21

that happened, I just could

2:24

not stop obsessing over

2:26

it. The worst experience

2:28

happened after I went on Gary

2:31

Vaynerchuk's podcast. So maybe you

2:33

know Gary's name he's a big celebrity

2:35

in the entrepreneurship space. He has a super

2:37

devoted fan base. And I I've

2:39

known Gary for years. I've interviewed him many

2:41

times. So here's how

2:43

he opened up the podcast. It was a very

2:45

natural way to do it. He explained that I'm

2:48

usually the one interviewing him, but now it's

2:50

a role reversal and he's interviewing me.

2:52

And then he said, welcome my friend

2:54

Jason to the show. Jeez, how are you?

2:56

Hey man. Great to see you. This is really fun

2:58

to be on the other side of this. III don't even

3:01

know how it's gonna go. I don't

3:03

even know how it's gonna go.

3:05

What are the words coming out of my mouth?

3:07

What am I talking about? and the

3:09

rest of the show just felt like that. Like,

3:12

I wanted to say something smart and instead

3:14

what came out of my mouth was just like a

3:16

b minus. I stories fell flat,

3:18

my points felt disjointed. I was not

3:20

as sharp as I wanted to be. I I could

3:22

feel it in the moment as it was happening.

3:24

And to be clear, Gary did nothing

3:27

wrong. He was welcoming and

3:29

engaging. I just could not find

3:31

my footing. I don't know what it was because I tired

3:33

that day? Did I have too many things on my mind?

3:36

And now, maybe you think, Jason, you are

3:38

being hard on yourself. This sounds

3:40

totally fine. And that is

3:42

true, rationally speaking.

3:45

But, you know, we are not dealing with

3:47

rational thought here. We are dealing with

3:49

something else. We are dealing with me. a

3:51

guy who is feeling a lot of pressure to maximize

3:53

every opportunity and who holds himself to

3:55

a very high standard. So when the interview with

3:57

Gary ended, Well,

4:00

we had taped it remotely, which meant that I

4:02

was then just sitting alone in my bedroom

4:04

and thinking, god.

4:06

I blew that. I blew a huge

4:08

chance to sell books and win over some of

4:10

Gary's audience. And then my

4:12

brain I just went, I don't

4:14

know. I guess it it just said

4:18

maybe it was fine. This is what my

4:20

brain is thought. Maybe it was fine. So I

4:22

tried to tell myself, I started to say out

4:24

loud. It was fine. It was fine.

4:26

It was fine. I started to pace in

4:28

my room talking to myself, repeating it. It

4:30

was fine. It was fine. I did this for minutes. It

4:32

was it was fine. It was fine. But I

4:34

was also thinking, no, it

4:36

was not fine. You could have told that story different.

4:38

could have made that point when he asked you that thing or

4:41

I don't know or maybe it was fine. It was fine.

4:43

And then I realized, oh, crap. I have to

4:45

run across town for this important meeting where I have

4:47

to be really on I am in no state to

4:49

do that right now because I'm assessing over this thing.

4:51

So I need to get this out my head. So I walked to

4:53

the subway and I was repeating it. It was fine. It was

4:55

fine. Then I'm on the subway. It was fine. It was

4:57

fine. It was No. It wasn't fine. Like, I'm literally

4:59

saying this out loud. I should've done this. I should've done

5:01

that. Maybe it was fine. I don't know. And then I thought,

5:03

okay. This this needs to

5:05

stop. Like, it needs to stop right now. This is crazy.

5:07

what else can I do here? So

5:10

I wondered, well,

5:12

what is happening to me right now?

5:14

Like, I've blown things

5:16

before and I felt bad about them, but

5:18

this feels like it's at a new level. I'm

5:20

not usually talking out loud to myself.

5:23

So Is this a thing that happens to

5:25

people? Is there a term for

5:27

what's happening to me? Is there scientific

5:29

literature on it? Because that

5:31

would make me feel less crazy, I

5:33

guess. So I started

5:35

googling around and soon I found

5:37

it. I found the term.

5:40

The term for what my brain

5:42

was doing. And it is

5:44

called counterfactual thinking.

5:47

Counterfactual thinking.

5:50

Turns out it's also very common.

5:53

And now that I knew what it

5:55

was, I could call people who study

5:57

it to ask what the hell is

5:59

going on

5:59

with me. which

6:00

is what I did. And so counterfactual thinking

6:03

is usually defined as simply

6:05

mentally simulating alternatives

6:09

to reality. and playing

6:11

them out and considering the

6:13

outcomes. In other words, it is

6:15

what if thinking? What if this

6:17

happened? What if I did that? Differently

6:19

comparing reality to an

6:21

imaginary world. Oh,

6:23

that voice you heard was John. John

6:25

Petricchioen? And I'm a social

6:28

psychologist. and professor at

6:30

Wake Forest University. And he specializes

6:32

in, among other things, counterfactual

6:35

thinking. My research has shown

6:37

that people are very, very

6:39

good at generating

6:41

these thoughts quite automatically. So

6:43

don't feel bad if if

6:45

you had the thoughts automatically and

6:47

you couldn't get rid of though. But here's the

6:49

interesting thing, which is going to sound obvious

6:51

at first, but is also pretty profound.

6:54

We also know that especially when

6:56

you're looking or you're hoping for a

6:58

desirable outcome. It's very easy

7:00

to undo just about

7:02

anything and to ventamentally

7:04

assume that it that it would have

7:06

changed the outcome in a in a

7:08

positive direction. Right. We

7:11

assume that if something had been

7:13

different in the situation in which we

7:15

are now regretting, it would have been different

7:17

in a better way. that

7:19

if only I'd said this or that thing on

7:21

the podcast that it would have been better, I would

7:23

have sold more books that if only you turned

7:26

left and said a right, you would have gotten that important

7:28

meeting on time that if only you'd stayed with that

7:30

girlfriend or boyfriend, your life would be

7:32

better and happier. If only,

7:34

if only, we think

7:36

we know what went wrong, and

7:38

therefore, we are sure about how it

7:40

could have gone right. But

7:42

that is wrong because

7:45

we don't know that at all. It's very

7:47

seductive and automatic and

7:49

can happen even at an implicit level.

7:51

We're not even aware of it. And shaping

7:54

our judgments and our decisions

7:56

in a way that we haven't really

7:58

fully explicitly

7:59

thought out. We may not have said.

8:02

We may not have Said

8:04

even in our self talk to ourselves,

8:06

we may not have written it down, but it could still

8:08

have quite the effect on

8:10

learning and memory and

8:12

decision making in the future. So

8:14

what is that effect? And

8:16

how can we shape it ourselves?

8:19

to gain some control back from the

8:21

counter factual thoughts to

8:23

counter the counter factuals. That

8:26

is what I went in search of. And I

8:28

have to say, The more I learned,

8:30

the better I felt, and I hope you can

8:32

feel the same. It's all coming up

8:34

after the break. Did you

8:36

know that the average podcast listener

8:38

subscribes to six shows. Well,

8:40

assuming you already subscribed to my show, then I

8:42

have a great reason for you to add another

8:44

one. It's called the Jordan

8:46

harbinger show, and honestly, Jordan is a

8:48

friend of mine, but I would recommend it even if

8:50

he wasn't because I love this

8:52

show and his interview style and just the

8:54

whole thing. Jordan dives into the

8:56

minds of fascinating people from athletes,

8:58

authors, and scientists to mobsters,

9:00

spies, and hostage negotiator He

9:02

has a real talent for drawing out never

9:04

heard before stories and thought provoking

9:07

insights, and he always pulls out tactical bits

9:09

of wisdom in each episode. all with noble

9:11

cause of making you more informed and

9:13

a better critical thinker. There's

9:15

good reason that Jordan is the guy I

9:17

text basically every time I have a question about

9:19

podcasting, and that's because he is the master

9:21

at it. Smart, funny, easy to listen to.

9:23

Check out his conversation with Tea Pain for a little

9:25

while ago. I have never heard the guy talk

9:27

like that. You'll be hard pressed to find an

9:29

episode without excellent conversation, a

9:31

few laughs and actionable advice that you can

9:33

directly use to improve your life.

9:35

So you can't go wrong with adding the Jordan

9:38

Harbinger show to your rotation. It's

9:40

incredibly interesting and there's never a dull

9:42

show. Search for the Jordan

9:44

Harbinger show that is 8ARB

9:46

as in boy, i, n as in

9:48

Nancy, GER on apple

9:50

podcasts, Spotify, or wherever you

9:52

listen to podcasts.

9:54

You know there's a fantastic person

9:56

out there who will improve your business. The

9:58

trick is to find them. For

10:00

a hiring partner that helps you reach

10:02

new heights, you need,

10:04

indeed. Indeed is the hiring

10:06

platform where you can attract, interview,

10:08

and hire all in one place. Don't

10:11

spend hours on multiple job sites looking

10:13

for candidates with the right skills when you

10:15

can do it all with indeed.

10:17

Find top talent fast with

10:19

indeed suite of hiring tools like

10:21

indeed instant match assessments and

10:23

virtual interviews. And if you wanna move

10:25

fast, well, good news.

10:27

Indeed, US data shows over eighty

10:29

percent of indeed employers find

10:31

quality candidates the moment they

10:33

sponsor a job. And

10:35

indeed knows that when you're growing your own business,

10:37

you have to make every dollar

10:39

count. That's why when you sponsor a

10:41

job, you only pay for quality

10:43

applications from resumes in the database

10:45

that match your job description. Visit

10:47

indeed dot com slash

10:49

archive to start hiring now.

10:51

Just go to indeed dot com

10:53

slash archive. Indeed

10:55

dot com slash archive.

10:57

Terms and conditions apply, cost per application

10:59

pricing not available for everyone, need to hire,

11:01

you need indeed.

11:04

Alright. We're back. So here's what we're gonna do. First,

11:06

we are going to understand more

11:08

about counter factual thinking. How

11:10

it happens? why it happens and

11:12

what we're missing when we're stuck

11:14

obsessing over what could have been.

11:16

And then we'll talk about what we

11:18

can actually do about to

11:20

try to unstuck that obsession.

11:22

Before we explain how it happens, let's explain what

11:25

it is.

11:25

That's a good plan. Okay? This

11:27

is Amy.

11:28

I'm Amy Somerville. I'm a social

11:30

psychology PhD who does

11:32

research on decision making and a version of the

11:34

special interest in how we think about what

11:36

might have Ben and the emotion of

11:38

regret.

11:38

So here's where to start according

11:40

to Amy. There are actually two

11:43

kinds of counter factual thinking and

11:45

we experience them at different times.

11:47

So you can

11:48

think about how things could have been

11:51

better and we call that upward counter factual

11:53

thinking, you're sort of looking up and imagining

11:55

how things could have been better.

11:56

That, of course, is what I was doing.

11:59

And

11:59

then there's also something called downward counterfactual

12:02

thinking. WHERE YOU THINK ABOUT HOW THINGS COULD

12:04

HAVE BEEN WORSE. WHEN I

12:05

FIRST HEARD THAT, I THOUGHT, WHY

12:08

WOULD ANYONE obsess OVER HOW THINGS COULD HAVE

12:10

BEEN WORSE I mean, if

12:12

you're feeling bad, it's all it's already

12:14

bad. And yet there are

12:16

many reasons. For example, survivors

12:18

of tragedy will keep THINKING ABOUT

12:20

HOW CLOSE THEY WERE TO INJURY OR

12:22

DEATH, BUT IT CAN HAPPEN MORE

12:24

CASUALLY.

12:24

THERE'S SOME REALLY INTERESTING WORKED BY

12:26

Laura CRAY AND BY appabriel that has

12:28

actually found that that downward counterfactual

12:31

thinking has some really interesting

12:33

value to us that it actually makes us feel that things

12:35

are more meant to be So if I

12:37

have you think about how, like, you might never

12:39

have met your best friend, that relationship

12:41

actually feels more meaningful to you. But,

12:43

okay,

12:43

we're gonna set all that aside now

12:45

because we're not actually here to talk about

12:48

downward counterfactuals today. We're

12:50

here to talk about upward counterfactuals.

12:52

Where Negative events.

12:54

prompt

12:54

us to think about what

12:56

could have been different. We want

12:58

to reinvent how could I have

13:01

avoided this bad thing.

13:02

But this doesn't happen all the time, of

13:05

course. Not every bad thing

13:07

leads to obsessive counter factual

13:09

thinking. So Why sometimes? What

13:11

is more likely to cause it? Amy

13:13

says that science has identified a bunch

13:15

of causes, and we're gonna talk about

13:17

three of them briefly here. The

13:19

first is proximity. Is there

13:21

something where something

13:23

was, you know, physically or numerically

13:25

or temporarily close For

13:27

example, people who miss a flight by

13:30

five minutes are more likely to

13:32

obsess over it than someone who missed a flight by an

13:34

hour. Because, you know, it was so

13:36

close they feel like had they just made

13:38

one different decision along the way

13:40

they could have made that flight. The

13:42

second common trigger is routine.

13:44

if we do something that's out of

13:47

routine or abnormal, that really

13:49

sticks out to us. So,

13:50

like, let's say you always prepare for

13:52

a meeting. And then one day you don't prepare and the

13:54

meeting was awful. So now you'll start to

13:56

think, if only I'd prepared, if

13:58

only I hadn't deviated from the

14:00

routine, And the third

14:02

trigger is if you are a person

14:04

who feels like they're in

14:06

control. There

14:07

does seem to be a link between feeling

14:09

a sense of head of personal agency

14:11

and control and feeling like

14:13

you're in charge of your own

14:15

destiny that does

14:18

relate to counter fact virtual thinking that you're

14:20

more likely to have these thoughts about, oh, I

14:22

could have done something different if you

14:24

have sort of that worldview. And for

14:25

what it's worth, sample size of one here.

14:28

But when I think back to what

14:30

triggered my own counterfactual thinking, I could

14:32

relate to all of that.

14:34

And here's another thing. Counterfactual

14:37

thinking does

14:39

not, by its very nature,

14:41

have to be bad or disruptive. It

14:43

can just be a thought So

14:45

what was I doing when I could not

14:47

get over it? Well, that's an added

14:49

layer on top. It's what psychologists

14:52

call illumination.

14:53

People can ruminate overall kinds of

14:55

things, not just counterfactuals. And the

14:57

word literally comes from how

14:59

cows digest their food, which

15:01

is that they vomit it back

15:03

up and chew it over again and again

15:05

and they keep so

15:07

it's super gross metaphor, but I

15:09

think actually really captures that

15:11

sense of something that's just sort of this

15:13

involuntary, intrusive, like,

15:16

it just keeps coming back up whether you

15:18

want it to or not.

15:19

So now let's get to the question that feels really

15:22

personal. Why do we do

15:24

this? I'd raised that question

15:26

earlier in the episode, but now it's time to go

15:28

deeper into it because When I spoke

15:30

with Amy, I had a theory.

15:32

I have this assumption, which could be an

15:34

incorrect one. But the assumption is that if there is

15:36

something common that happens in

15:38

our brain, then there was

15:40

some kind of evolutionary purpose

15:42

to it even if it doesn't get expressed

15:45

in maybe the way in which it was supposed to

15:47

be. And and so my best guests

15:49

here with with counterfactual thinking or

15:51

or illumination, I suppose, is is that

15:53

the reason that we do this is because we're

15:55

programmed to learn. And so we go through

15:57

experiences, and then we're trying to take the

15:59

lessons from those experiences. But the

16:01

problem is that if if those

16:03

experiences don't match up our expectations, then

16:05

we're in a kind of loop where we

16:08

realize that we maybe have learned a hard

16:10

lesson, but it feels very bad. And

16:12

so what we end up doing is kind of going back and trying

16:14

to, like, wheel the past

16:16

into some kind of different

16:18

experience. Am I am I right

16:20

there? What do we know about why this

16:22

is a thing that happens? Howard Bauchner:

16:24

Yeah, so you're you're exactly right.

16:26

So those of us who have what we call sort of

16:28

a functional view of counterfactuals

16:31

believe exactly that. The counterfactuals are

16:33

what help us learn from our mistakes.

16:36

And my colleague Rachel Smallman has

16:38

done a lot of really interesting work showing

16:40

that there's actually a link between counterfactuals

16:43

and forming intentions for the

16:45

future. So, you know, if you

16:47

were walking around with your coffee this

16:49

morning and you spilled it on yourself

16:51

and you say, ah, I like I should have put

16:53

a lid on my coffee or I should have

16:55

used a travel log instead of an

16:56

open mug, then you're more

16:59

likely tomorrow morning to say, oh, let me put

17:01

that lid on at the hotel or let me grab my

17:03

travel mug out of the cabinet instead of my open mug

17:05

at home. and therefore avoided

17:07

in the future. But there's

17:08

another camp of thinkers and

17:10

they say no counterfactuals

17:13

are not a byproduct of the way that we learn. They

17:15

are instead a byproduct of how

17:17

we make sense of the world, which is

17:20

what drives a lot of our biases and

17:22

misunderstanding. Like, we

17:24

want

17:24

the world to seem controllable

17:26

and to have the world seem like it

17:28

makes causal sense. was

17:30

really kind of psychologically threatening to think that,

17:32

like, just bad things happen randomly. And

17:35

so sometimes counterfactuals can be

17:37

used just as a way of

17:39

kind of telling a story that

17:41

makes the world seem

17:44

more kind of sensible. For

17:47

example, Amy says that

17:47

you can see this in victim blaming. So

17:49

let's say someone forgets to lock their

17:51

front door one night and that their home gets

17:54

broken into. other people might think,

17:56

well, if they locked their front

17:58

door, the home wouldn't have been burglarized.

18:00

So it was their fault, which

18:03

maybe, maybe not. But what's

18:05

really happening here is that those other people

18:07

are trying to make sense of this

18:09

story in a world in which they do

18:11

not want to be the person. that bad things happen

18:13

to. So they wanna transform this

18:15

from a story about random

18:17

chance where a bad person just

18:19

decided to do a bad thing

18:21

to a random person into a story

18:23

about what could have been done to

18:25

avoid it because that feels

18:28

safer. random chance is

18:30

scary. And this is actually one of the

18:32

great dangers of counterfactuals because

18:34

even if counterfactuals are a means

18:36

of us learning from experience, it's

18:39

also very hard to know what

18:41

lesson we're supposed to learn from any

18:43

one situation. you

18:44

can focus on the wrong

18:46

specific behavior because

18:49

it's unknowable. Like, which of these

18:51

specific behaviors would

18:53

have changed things.

18:54

Maybe locking the front door would have deterred

18:57

the burglar or maybe not because

18:59

if the door was locked, they would have just broken through a

19:01

window. We don't know. we

19:03

often can't know. Amy

19:05

used a simpler example to illustrate

19:07

this. You heard it a second ago. She was

19:09

saying, you know, you're carrying a cup of coffee and you

19:11

spill it on yourself. So what are you

19:13

gonna learn from it? You know, you're you're probably gonna think,

19:15

well, if I did just if I

19:17

only didn't do this one thing, I wouldn't have

19:20

spilled the coffee. But Alright. One

19:22

thing. To not drink coffee, to

19:24

not walk with coffee, to not have a

19:26

lid on your coffee, to not fill the cup up

19:28

as much as you did, as it turns out,

19:30

Amy says, unless a bad

19:32

outcome is very clear,

19:34

like touching a hot stove and getting

19:36

burned, then people aren't

19:38

actually very good at identifying the thing that

19:40

led to a bad outcome. We

19:42

know that

19:42

there are biases in the way that

19:45

people tend to come up with

19:47

these things. So we talked about this

19:49

idea of, you know, what's your

19:51

routine and people tend to focus on

19:53

the things that are out of routine. People

19:55

also tend to focus on

19:57

things

19:57

that kind of happened early or

20:00

late in a string of events. So

20:02

I grew up in Indiana, so basketball was

20:04

basically a state religion. And you see

20:06

this all the time. Right? Like, there there is the

20:08

guy who misses the shot right at the

20:10

buzzer and, you know, they were

20:12

down by one. And if he'd made it, then they

20:14

would have won. And, ah, like, if he just

20:16

made that shot. But of course, every

20:18

single missed shot has exactly the

20:20

same impact on the score. Right? If the guy who,

20:22

in the middle of the third quarter, had

20:24

made that shot too, That also

20:26

would have came to the outcome of the game

20:28

by exactly the game. They could have

20:29

they could have about ten instead of down one at the end

20:31

of that game if a couple other people had made

20:33

shots. Exactly.

20:34

Yeah. So if you

20:35

can't quite identify the thing that would

20:38

have made the difference, then you're

20:40

gonna do one of two things. Either

20:42

number one, you're

20:44

going to learn nothing from this thing that you're

20:46

obsessing over. Or number

20:48

two, you will potentially draw the

20:50

wrong lesson. And perhaps

20:52

this is why people do not learn

20:54

from experience nearly

20:55

as quickly as we would hope and

20:58

and think they would.

21:00

That is John Petachelli from Wake Forest

21:02

again. For example, he says okay.

21:04

So he describes the study in which

21:06

people are asked to make a decision based

21:08

on coin flips. and what

21:10

they don't realize is that the coin is

21:13

weighted so that it ends up landing

21:15

twice as much on heads as it does

21:17

on tails. Most people

21:19

think that People would learn

21:21

that quite readily because heads is

21:23

coming up twice for

21:25

every one tails on

21:27

average. But it takes quite a while

21:29

for people to learn that maybe upwards

21:31

of sixty flips if they're even

21:33

paying attention. Why? Again,

21:35

too many factors. you're not paying close

21:38

attention, you think it's random chance, you're

21:40

focusing on other things happening in the study,

21:42

and this is a simple scenario

21:44

a coin flip has a very limited number

21:46

of factors. Now, make that

21:48

more complex, like why you blew it

21:50

on a date or why you blew that

21:53

meeting or why I blew those podcasts.

21:55

Maybe you've done forty podcasts

21:57

and all all other, you know, thirty nine or

21:59

thirty eight of them have gone well. It's

22:02

very difficult and say, well, I'm gonna learn

22:04

from this one decision. It's

22:06

very seductive to think I'm going to learn

22:08

from it, but But chances are if you're in the same

22:11

exact context, you're gonna make the same

22:13

decision. And then maybe

22:15

even ruminate over the

22:17

same counterfactuals. So

22:19

this doesn't sound very promising. Does

22:22

it? I mean, here we're trapped in a kind of

22:24

thinking that maybe helps us

22:26

learn or maybe just helps us

22:28

tell comforting fantasies about the world.

22:31

Either way, we're so bad at learning

22:33

from experience that we're doomed to repeat the

22:35

same mistakes we regret anyway. or

22:37

are we? There is

22:40

actually some hope for us yet.

22:42

I promise. And that is what's

22:44

coming up after the break.

22:47

If you are an entrepreneurial minded

22:49

person, if you are always thinking, what's

22:51

the next big idea for a business or

22:53

just even a side hustle, Well then, I've got a

22:55

podcast for you. Listen to

22:57

my first million. The two

22:59

hosts Sean, Perry, and

23:01

Sampar have both started and sold their own businesses

23:03

to Amazon and HubSpot. Every

23:05

week, they brainstorm business ideas that

23:07

you can start Tomorrow. These can

23:09

be little side hustles that make you ten grand

23:11

a month or big ideas that could be a billion

23:14

dollar thing. They also chat with

23:16

entrepreneurs and invest celebrities and

23:18

billionaires about business ideas and trends they're

23:20

investing in. I recommend checking out their

23:22

recent episode with Andrew Wilkinson,

23:24

where he talks about why getting rich

23:26

slow is better than getting rich quick.

23:28

They also talk about the five pillars of

23:30

happiness, how to identify winners, and

23:32

much more. These are the kinds of conversations

23:34

they can get you thinking big

23:37

and then acting big. So check it out.

23:39

Just search for my first

23:41

million on Apple Podcasts,

23:43

Spotify, or wherever you listen

23:45

to podcasts.

23:45

I am

23:48

moving homes right now, so I'm thinking

23:50

a lot about what it takes to create

23:52

a comfortable welcoming space. And

23:54

that's why I really love Jenny Kane

23:56

Home because it has everything you

23:58

need to create the home

23:59

of your dreams in stock and

24:02

ready to ship. Jenny

24:04

Kain Homes is the ultimate source for

24:06

all things home. The sofa, throws, or

24:08

even the candles make rooms feel

24:11

complete. And if you want to experience the best of

24:13

Jenny Kain home, then check out the

24:15

Jenny Kain home membership.

24:17

For just one hundred and fifty dollars

24:19

annually, you can enjoy insider

24:21

only perks that get you one step closer

24:23

to your dream space, including

24:25

twenty percent off on all furniture

24:27

and home décor, complimentary

24:30

styling services, free shipping on

24:32

furniture, early product access, and

24:34

much more. Join at

24:36

jenny kane dot com slash

24:38

membership or go to jenny

24:40

kane dot com slash home to create the space you'll

24:42

never want to leave. That's

24:44

JENNIKAYNE

24:49

dot com slash home.

24:51

Ari, we're

24:53

back. And now it is time

24:55

to talk solutions. although let's

24:58

set some expectations here.

25:00

When I asked Amy if there was some

25:02

kind of simple exercise that people

25:04

could do to escape counterfactuals,

25:07

said I don't

25:08

know that there is. Because,

25:09

you know, when we become the human

25:12

equivalent of the spinning pinwheel

25:14

of death on a screen, you can't

25:16

just reboot the system. Instead,

25:18

the starting point has to be understanding

25:20

what is happening in your head

25:22

and then using that awareness to

25:25

your advantage. Like, for example, to

25:27

go back to one of the things that Amy said

25:29

could trigger counterfactuals.

25:30

We know that people

25:32

tend to feel the most

25:35

ongoing regret about the parts of their life

25:37

where they feel a

25:38

sense of control and they feel like things

25:40

are important. And so I think partially

25:43

just recognizing that, right, that

25:45

you you're feeling this

25:47

big regret in the moment because

25:49

this is something that is gonna continue

25:51

to matter to you. Right? If it

25:53

was not important, you would just get

25:55

over it. And so I think

25:57

accepting that that's just the price of

25:59

caring a

25:59

little bit.

26:00

I really like that phrase, the price

26:03

of caring. And it

26:05

reminded me of something that my wife often

26:07

tells me when I get into moments like this, which

26:09

is basically like Nobody is thinking

26:11

about this as much as you are, she tells me.

26:13

Not the podcast host, not the

26:15

podcast listeners. Nobody. So

26:17

I told Amy what my wife

26:19

tries to do is to get

26:21

me to care a little less. Right? To

26:23

say, you know what? Fine. Even

26:25

if it was bad, what does it matter?

26:27

because people will forget it instantly.

26:29

You didn't do something terrible. You just

26:31

didn't perform as well as you thought you would. Right?

26:33

Like, nobody's gonna write about how terrible you

26:36

were on Gary V's podcast. they're just

26:38

gonna take it as it is. And so

26:40

maybe just care a little less or

26:42

put it in perspective about how how it's not

26:44

it's not

26:44

important Yeah. One of the things you were saying, you know, that your wife might

26:47

tell you of, like, people are gonna forget this

26:49

in five minutes. I

26:50

you think think is

26:51

that idea of taking kind of a longer

26:53

kind of temple oral time. So

26:56

there's a lot of work on this

26:58

idea that there are real

27:00

differences in how we think about things that

27:02

are close to

27:04

us physically in time or things

27:06

that are far away. So this comes up, you

27:08

may have heard of, like, the delayed discounting effect.

27:10

So this is something where

27:12

If I tell you, I'll give you a dollar

27:15

right now, or I'll give

27:17

you five bucks a week from now.

27:19

Most people take the dollar right now. But I

27:21

say, hey, I'll give you a dollar

27:23

a year from now, or

27:25

I'll give you five dollars a year and

27:27

a week from most people take

27:29

the five bucks. Because when you're

27:31

really thinking about right now, like, right now, like, yeah,

27:34

like a buck would be great. I could go down the

27:36

hall. I could get a out of the vending

27:38

machine, that'd be really nice before I go on stage. There's sort

27:40

of this closeness to it

27:42

versus when you think about a year

27:44

from now, right,

27:45

that difference of a week feels super abstract. Like,

27:47

of course, I'd take five, dollars that's totally

27:49

the rational choice here. And so

27:51

I think that taking that kind of

27:54

long view about your own regrets as

27:56

well. There

27:56

are some things where you

27:58

make a decision now that five years

28:00

from now, you are still gonna be thinking about. that

28:03

decision. Like, our decisions have

28:05

consequences. But there are also

28:07

some things where five

28:09

years from now, you're not even gonna

28:11

remember what it is upset about right now. And so I

28:13

think really trying to

28:16

triage on that is really helpful.

28:18

So kind of taking the the long

28:20

view of Okay, five years

28:21

from now, what is it that you think you

28:23

should have learned from this moment? You know,

28:26

five years from now, is this even a thing

28:28

you're gonna care about.

28:30

I think is probably one of

28:32

the best things you could

28:33

try. That's awesome. I'll tell you that what up

28:35

being the Cure for me was the

28:37

podcast coming out. Because when it came out

28:39

and people didn't react negatively to

28:42

it. Right? I mean, maybe they could have

28:44

reacted more positively to it.

28:46

I don't know. But that's now that's a level of

28:48

abstraction that I sort of can't can't

28:50

deal with. Right? But when when

28:52

I got messages from strangers who said that

28:54

they they liked that episode.

28:56

I could just feel that tension

28:58

immediately disappear from my body.

29:00

It's

29:00

very good. We started up by talking about

29:02

Upward and downward counterfactuals and

29:05

and it's really all very

29:07

relative. One of the great examples

29:08

of this is there's a lot of evidence

29:10

showing that if you show people

29:12

the faces of Olympic Medalists and

29:14

have them rate how happy or

29:17

upset these medalists look on the medalist.

29:20

Silver medalists are consistently

29:22

the least happy people on

29:24

the podium. because they're thinking

29:26

about I could have won gold

29:28

and actually bronze medalists tend to

29:30

be substantially happier than silver

29:32

medalist because they're thinking I

29:34

want a medal. Like, I almost didn't win a medal, and I

29:36

won an Olympic medal. That's amazing. And so

29:38

I think that, you know, trying to take the view

29:40

of, like, remembering that as

29:42

a actually objectively better off in the

29:44

bronze medalist. You know, is also, I

29:47

think, a really helpful thing.

29:49

So know,

29:50

yes, maybe the podcast could have gone better,

29:52

but like, wow, you were a guest

29:54

on a podcast that you

29:56

really admired. And you know, that was

29:58

an amazing opportunity

29:59

and focus on

30:02

that thing that was good about

30:04

the situation. and

30:06

really kind of keep that comparison point in

30:08

perspective.

30:08

That's amazing. I love that.

30:10

But we could go one level deeper

30:13

because here's the thing. counterfactuals can

30:15

trap us in a bad way of

30:17

thinking, but counterfactuals might

30:19

also be our escape route.

30:21

Here is John again. Well, the

30:23

trick is And it's gonna sound a little

30:25

funny, but the trick is to

30:27

consider additional alternatives,

30:29

to consider other counter factual.

30:32

Here's why. John reminded me of something

30:34

that psychologists call the availability

30:38

heuristic, which basically means that we are

30:40

biased towards the stuff that's easiest

30:42

to remember. This came up a few months ago in

30:44

an episode of this podcast that I

30:46

titled All The Fund Facts You

30:49

Have Wrong. where we misinformation

30:51

spreads in part because lies

30:53

often feel true and are just

30:55

easy to remember. That is

30:57

the availability heuristic. So

30:59

the easier it is to generate a thought or

31:02

the easier it is to generate examples

31:05

of an event or an examples of an

31:07

argument you're trying to make the

31:09

easier it is, the more likely we think

31:11

it is to be true, or the

31:13

more likely we think it is to occur. And

31:15

if that's the case, then every

31:18

time we run through a counterfactual in our

31:20

head, the idea becomes even

31:22

more compelling and convincing to us.

31:24

It feels truer through

31:26

repetition. So instead of doing that, instead

31:28

of convincing ourselves through repetition, we

31:30

should instead add noise,

31:33

add more things to consider or

31:35

fight the counterfactuals with counterfactuals. If

31:38

you open your mind to them, it's much

31:40

easier to see, well, you know what?

31:42

Maybe it wasn't such a bad podcast

31:45

because it could have been a hell of a lot

31:47

worse, which I admit

31:50

is true. I mean, a b performance is

31:52

still better than a total

31:54

embarrassment. I don't know who first said it. I think it's

31:56

been around probably for at

31:58

least two centuries, the better to

32:00

have loved and lost than

32:02

to never have loved at all. Right? Would

32:04

you have rather not done that pot

32:06

cast at all. I mean, that's another alternative.

32:10

Right? At least you did it. You know,

32:12

at least you you got it out there

32:14

and once you expand those

32:16

possibilities. It's easier to kind of give yourself a break.

32:18

I didn't know who said that either, but

32:20

I got curious. So I looked it up. It's

32:22

often attributed to Shakespeare, but it's

32:25

actually from the English poet Lord Alfred

32:27

Tennison in an eighteen fifty poem

32:29

of his called Inn memoriam

32:32

AHH The poem

32:34

was about the death of his friend Arthur

32:36

Henry Hallum, AHH

32:39

Poitt who died of a stroke at age twenty too.

32:41

The original line was almost exactly as

32:43

people say it today. T is better

32:45

to have loved and lost than never

32:47

to have loved at all. But

32:49

anyway, we digress. The point here is there

32:51

are a lot of things that we can tell

32:54

ourselves. And just because we tell ourselves

32:56

one thing, doesn't make it true.

32:58

That actually was the biggest takeaway

33:00

for me in these conversations.

33:02

You know, both Amy and John made

33:04

this point to me I referenced

33:06

it earlier in the episode, we

33:08

base counterfactuals on a belief that if

33:10

we just did one thing different,

33:13

everything would have been better. But

33:15

we don't actually know that. It may

33:17

not be true. Everything we do is

33:19

the product of a million random

33:22

actors, only some of which are in our control.

33:24

Amy compared it to an entrepreneur whose

33:26

business fails and who then beats themselves up

33:28

by thinking, if only I'd

33:31

made this decision. If only I'd done that partnership,

33:33

if only I'd hired this

33:35

other person, then the business would

33:37

have survived. And, you know,

33:39

know Maybe that's true. But

33:42

also, maybe it's not. A lot of

33:43

dentures failed, not because people made bad

33:46

choices, not because people were bad

33:48

entrepreneurs, but

33:50

Things failed because there were these massive forces

33:52

outside of anybody's control and

33:54

ability to predict. Run the scenario

33:56

ten thousand times and

33:59

make slightly different decisions each time, and you still

34:01

might end up with ten thousand

34:03

failures. Not everything is a

34:05

recipe for success. Sometimes,

34:07

it just wasn't your day. I

34:09

have to say, terrible as that

34:12

all sounds. The inevitability of

34:14

failure actually gives me a

34:16

little comfort It's like, I

34:18

don't know, I feel like I have

34:20

control over most things in my life and

34:22

that's why when something doesn't break my

34:24

way, I beat myself up over it. You know, I figure, I was in

34:26

control. How did I mess it up? But

34:28

if we can't control everything,

34:30

then maybe

34:32

it's maybe it's

34:34

not worth beating ourselves

34:36

up over. I know I'm probably

34:38

teetering into cliche territory here

34:40

if I'm not already there a long shot,

34:42

but it makes me think, you know,

34:44

like every all star basketball player

34:46

missed a game winning shot. every

34:50

meticulously prepared political candidate

34:52

still bombs in a debate. If

34:54

our definition of success is complete perfection,

34:56

though we are only setting ourselves

34:58

up for failure because nothing is perfect.

35:00

No matter how much we

35:02

think we know about how to

35:06

create perfection. So where does that leave me? Well, it leaves

35:08

me thinking about this question that

35:10

I heard somebody say a long time

35:12

ago on a

35:14

podcast. I wish I could remember who

35:16

it was so I could credit them. But anyway, they were saying that look, if

35:18

we ask the question, is this

35:22

perfect? about the things that we are doing

35:24

or evaluating or wondering whether

35:26

they're worth our time or

35:28

effort? Well, then

35:30

we already have our answer because nothing is perfect. So you ask

35:32

the question, is this perfect? And the answer

35:35

is no. And if that's the

35:38

filter through which we're going to push things, then we're going to

35:40

filter almost everything out.

35:42

But what if instead we asked

35:45

a different question? what if instead

35:47

we ask, is this new

35:50

problem better

35:51

than our old problem?

35:54

Because when you ask

35:56

that, well, you just leave open the reality of problems.

35:58

Problems are part of the process.

35:59

And therefore, the problem

36:02

doesn't scare you

36:04

off. It's just well,

36:06

you know, it's just the cost of

36:08

admission. And also, that means that you can

36:10

then track progress

36:13

through problems. to whole embarrassing thing with me and

36:15

the Gary Faynerchuk podcast. And I think, well,

36:17

you know, the podcast

36:20

came out and people listened

36:22

to it, and a bunch of people actually reached out

36:24

to tell me that they really liked

36:26

it. And I still think

36:28

I could have done a better job,

36:30

but, you know, Is this

36:32

a better problem than another

36:34

problem? The old problem, I guess, was

36:36

that I wasn't on Gary Vaynerchuk's podcast.

36:39

Now I have been and I reached some

36:42

people. And even though I didn't do it

36:44

perfectly, I guess that's a better

36:46

problem, isn't it? That is

36:48

how I'm going to try to look at things from now

36:50

on and maybe you want to

36:52

too. Counterfactual

36:54

thinking, terrifying is it can feel is also a good

36:56

problem to have because it

36:58

means we did something meaningful. It

37:00

means we had something on

37:02

the line It means we

37:04

tried hard and are committed to

37:06

trying again and maybe we'll

37:08

even learn something. Because as

37:10

the old poet said,

37:12

have podcasted and delivered a

37:14

mediocre performance

37:16

than to never have podcasted at all.

37:19

And that's our episode. Now,

37:22

here's a question you did not expect

37:24

or maybe you did, but that would be really

37:26

weird. Here's the question. do

37:28

humans and cockroaches have in

37:30

common when it comes to performing

37:32

tasks? This came up in

37:34

my research search for this episode, and I will tell you the answer in a

37:36

minute. But first, if you

37:38

are going through a big change

37:40

at work

37:42

or in your life right now, then you need a copy of

37:44

my new book. It is called Build

37:46

For Tomorrow, just like this podcast.

37:50

It combines lessons from this podcast with lessons from the smartest

37:52

entrepreneurs of today and the history of

37:54

innovation and provides a step

37:56

by step action plan for how you

37:58

can thrive in changing

37:59

times and find opportunity

38:02

in adversity. It is available in

38:04

hardcover audiobook and ebooks,

38:06

so just go wherever you find any

38:08

of that or to jason pfeiffer

38:10

dot com slash book. And if

38:12

you want even more advice and encouragement

38:14

on how to adapt fast, then

38:16

sign up for my newsletter. Find it by

38:19

going to jason pfeiffer dot com slash newsletter. You can also get

38:21

in touch with me directly at my

38:23

website, jason pfeiffer dot com or follow

38:25

me on Twitter or

38:27

in Instagram, I am at haypfeiffer.

38:30

This episode was reported and

38:32

written by me, Jason Feiffer, Sound editing

38:34

by Alec Bayless, Our theme music is

38:36

my Casper baby pants. Learn more at

38:38

baby pants music dot com.

38:40

Thanks to Adam Sokulek for production

38:42

help. This show is supported in part

38:44

by the stand together trust. The stand together trust

38:46

believes that advances in technology have

38:48

transformed society for the better and is looking

38:50

to support

38:52

scholars, policy experts, and other projects and creators

38:54

who focus on embracing innovation, creating

38:56

a society that fosters innovation and

38:59

encouraging people to engineer the next great idea. If

39:01

that's you, then get in touch with them.

39:04

Proposals for projects in law, economics,

39:06

history, political science and philosophy

39:08

are in courage. To learn

39:10

more about their partnership criteria,

39:12

visit stand together trust

39:14

dot org. Alright. Now,

39:16

as promised, let's talk about cockroaches.

39:19

As I spoke with John about counter factual thinking, we got

39:22

off on a tangent about

39:24

performance because as you heard earlier, we tend

39:26

to counter factualize things

39:28

where we feel like we're in control or have a routine. And

39:30

that led John to tell me a few

39:32

crazy things about what improves people's

39:34

performance in high

39:36

stress situations. including

39:38

if you have a well practiced

39:40

skill or behavior and you

39:42

have an audience, people tend to

39:44

to perform better. that's an old sort

39:46

of social facilitation theory. It was

39:49

even demonstrated with cockroaches that

39:52

that cockroaches ran faster mazes

39:55

to the food when they were being

39:58

watched. Why other

39:59

cockroaches in another container that that

40:02

they could get, you know, that they could

40:04

conceivably see So

40:05

the next time the pressure's on and people

40:07

are watching, just think to

40:10

yourself, I will do this

40:12

like a cockroach. so

40:14

inspirational. Hey, that's the end of

40:16

this episode. I hope I won't suffer any

40:19

counterfactual thinking after I publish

40:21

it, but there is only one way to find out,

40:23

so here it goes. Thanks for listening.

40:25

I'm Jason Pfeiffer, and let's keep

40:27

building for tomorrow.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features