Podchaser Logo
Home
David Charbonneau and Doris Tsao: Looking for Something Familiar

David Charbonneau and Doris Tsao: Looking for Something Familiar

Released Wednesday, 12th June 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
David Charbonneau and Doris Tsao: Looking for Something Familiar

David Charbonneau and Doris Tsao: Looking for Something Familiar

David Charbonneau and Doris Tsao: Looking for Something Familiar

David Charbonneau and Doris Tsao: Looking for Something Familiar

Wednesday, 12th June 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

There are any number of reasons you might consider

0:03

selling your home. To move closer to family,

0:05

live within a smaller budget, or just wanting

0:07

a change of scenery. Whatever your

0:09

reasons, having to figure out all the various

0:11

housing market trends in your area may not

0:13

be what you signed up for. That's where an

0:15

agent who is a Realtor comes in. Realtors

0:18

have the expertise to help you find the right

0:20

price and navigate the process to sell your home

0:22

in a way that's right for you. That's

0:26

who we are. Realtors are

0:28

members of the National

0:30

Association of Realtors. 40,000.

0:33

That's the average number of questions children

0:35

ask between two and five years old.

0:37

At the Goddard School, we say bring

0:40

them on because in our classrooms, children

0:42

take the lead by asking questions, making

0:44

new discoveries, and embracing the wonder of

0:46

learning. We offer 52 weeks

0:49

of lessons, each customized to your

0:51

child's unique interests. With passionate teachers

0:53

guiding children every step of the

0:56

way, the Goddard School is where

0:58

extraordinary awaits. Visit goddardschool.com to learn

1:00

more. This

1:03

program is sponsored by the

1:05

Cauvly Prize, which honors scientists

1:07

for breakthroughs in astrophysics, nanoscience,

1:10

and neuroscience. The

1:12

Cauvly Prize is a partnership among

1:14

the Norwegian Academy of Science and

1:16

Letters, the Norwegian Ministry of Education

1:18

and Research, and the

1:20

U.S.-based Cauvly Foundation in Los

1:22

Angeles, California. I'm

1:29

Alan Alder, and this is Clear

1:31

and Vivid, conversations about

1:34

connecting and communicating. This

1:40

morning, June 12th, the winners of the 2024 Cauvly Prize

1:42

were announced.

1:45

There are eight winners in all, and in this

1:47

episode of Clear and Vivid, I'll

1:49

be talking with two of them. David

1:51

Charbonneau is an astrophysicist searching

1:53

for planets around other suns,

1:56

and he's a pioneer in developing a way

1:58

to discover if these any- exoplanets may

2:01

support life. Doris

2:03

Tso is a neuroscientist building on

2:05

her work exploring the brain's ability

2:07

to recognize faces in

2:10

order to understand how we recognize anything,

2:12

including, as we'll find out,

2:15

fire hydrants. First,

2:17

my conversation with David Charbonneau. You

2:21

know, I'm very interested in

2:23

how excitement spreads among scientists

2:26

and propels science forward. Sarah

2:28

Seager has been a

2:30

guest on our show and she's your

2:32

fellow cavalry laureate. Yes. And

2:34

I heard a story in which her

2:36

excitement propelled you on your life's work.

2:39

It's absolutely true. Sarah and I

2:41

knew each other since

2:44

before I was an astronomer. She

2:46

knew me when I was a

2:48

teenager and I had

2:50

just started at the University of Toronto and

2:52

we met because we were both very interested

2:54

in hiking and canoeing in

2:57

the Canadian wilderness. And

2:59

so we would organize trips for other

3:02

students at the university who were interested in

3:05

going out and exploring on the weekend. And

3:08

then I learned she was interested in

3:10

mathematics and physics. And then

3:12

several years later, she went off to graduate school

3:15

in the United States at Harvard and

3:18

she wrote and said, gosh, this

3:20

is a great program. You should apply here

3:22

when you graduate. And so I did.

3:24

And it's been a really nice

3:26

connection because she shifted into exoplanets

3:29

and I moved into the field of

3:31

exoplanets which was a completely new

3:33

field. And she works

3:35

on the theoretical side. She makes

3:37

predictions and calculations and

3:40

I'm very much on the observational side trying

3:42

to measure and test some of those theories.

3:46

So exoplanets, these planets that

3:49

are outside of our solar system

3:51

and orbiting other stars, they're

3:53

hard to detect, right? They're very small

3:55

and very far away. Yes,

3:58

the trick with trying to... find a planet around

4:01

another star is that, of course, the star puts

4:03

out an enormous amount of light and the planet

4:05

doesn't put out much at all. And

4:08

so the big realization was to

4:10

use the star as an ally

4:13

instead. And we look at

4:15

the light of the star, and if

4:17

the planet happens to pass in front of the

4:19

star every time that it makes an orbit, it

4:21

blocks some of that light, and that's relatively easy

4:23

to measure. So that's when

4:26

I was a graduate student, I

4:28

was the first person to make that measurement to see one

4:30

of these eclipses, which we

4:33

now call transits. So

4:35

Sarah's excitement transferred to you and

4:37

you right away found your first

4:39

one. That's amazing. Yeah,

4:41

it was a big step forward in terms

4:43

of our understanding of these worlds, because for

4:46

the first time, we could

4:49

learn both their mass and

4:51

their size. And if you have a sphere, if

4:53

you have a planet and you know how much

4:55

it weighs and how big it is, you can

4:58

calculate its density. And of course, that's

5:00

your first guess at what it's actually made of.

5:02

And so that particular planet had

5:05

a very low density and we learned it was

5:07

a gas giant, it was similar to Jupiter. But

5:11

since then, this technique has been

5:13

used to discover and characterize thousands

5:15

of planets. And of course,

5:17

the measurements have gotten much, much better. And so now

5:19

we're able to do this regularly for

5:21

Earth-sized planets and really see that indeed they're

5:23

made of rock and iron, just like our

5:25

own planet. What

5:28

we realized after we had a transiting

5:30

planet was that

5:32

we could use the light from the

5:34

star to probe the planetary atmosphere. And

5:37

so we applied

5:40

for and we're awarded time on

5:42

the Hubble Space Telescope, because of

5:44

course, we want to be free of our own atmosphere if

5:46

we're trying to study the atmosphere of another planet. We want

5:49

to be looking up for our own messy atmosphere. There's

5:51

enough going on there already. You say,

5:53

well, look at all that oxygen there

5:55

and it's large. Well, yeah, that's right.

5:57

Now, for your jumping ahead. That's

6:00

what we want to do soon. But for

6:02

the first planet, we

6:05

waited for it to pass in front of its star.

6:08

And then we looked very carefully with the

6:10

Hubble Space Telescope. And we were

6:12

able to basically take spectra

6:14

of the star, so measure how bright

6:17

the star is in different colors. And

6:19

of course, different atoms and molecules basically

6:23

leave a fingerprint. They

6:25

block very specific colors of

6:27

light. And

6:30

that method, that idea of

6:32

letting the planet pass in front of the star and

6:34

studying what's in its

6:36

atmosphere by studying how the light

6:38

is transmitted through the planetary atmosphere,

6:40

that I think really is our

6:43

first big shot to go

6:45

and look for biosignatures, to go and

6:47

look for things like oxygen, which

6:50

are actually made by life on those

6:52

planets, and maybe infer the presence of

6:54

life indirectly by that

6:56

method. Is there any other reason

6:58

oxygen would be in the atmosphere other

7:00

than life put it there? Oh,

7:03

gosh, yeah. There's been a lot of work

7:05

thinking, if

7:07

you detect oxygen, can you really

7:09

say that it's due to life? And

7:13

one idea is, OK, maybe if there's a

7:15

lot of water on the planet and

7:17

the planet's very close to its star, then

7:20

the UV radiation from the star

7:22

will break apart the water. And as you know,

7:24

water is made from hydrogen and oxygen. And so

7:26

you would make a lot of oxygen gas that

7:28

way. So

7:31

there's a lot of work done

7:33

to think about these false positives.

7:36

And I think what we've learned as

7:38

a community from those studies is that

7:40

just detecting oxygen isn't enough. But

7:43

if you were able to detect

7:45

that there was oxygen, there

7:47

was carbon dioxide and methane, and

7:50

measure the relative amounts of other

7:52

molecules at the same time, then

7:55

really the only plausible explanation

7:57

would be life, in particular

7:59

would be photosynthesis. Because all

8:01

of those together. All those together, that's...

8:03

Kind of signature of life. Exactly,

8:05

exactly. And so that's really where the community

8:08

is headed now. On

8:10

a maybe a 10-year or 15-year timescale to

8:12

really go after trying to measure all of

8:15

those things together and really look

8:17

for life on

8:19

a distant world through the detection of

8:21

these biosignatures. I take it

8:23

from what you're saying now, there hasn't been a planet

8:25

found yet. Would

8:28

that combination, is that what you're saying? Exactly.

8:30

So no one has yet

8:33

detected molecular oxygen in

8:35

the atmosphere of a planet

8:37

orbiting of a star. Let alone

8:39

seen it in combination with these other

8:41

gases that we know are present on

8:43

the Earth. But we know

8:46

how to do it. We know exactly what

8:48

colors of light you would have to look

8:50

for and how big a telescope you would

8:52

need. And so we have a plan

8:54

in the works as a community about how to

8:56

do it. A big part of my work

8:58

is thinking about and building

9:01

telescopes to discover these planets that in

9:04

the future we will want to go

9:06

and characterize. And so a

9:09

project that I have put a lot of

9:11

time into over the last decade is

9:13

called the Mirth Project. And the reason

9:15

we call it the Mirth Project is because...

9:19

Okay, it makes us happy. So Mirth. It's

9:21

Mirth, yeah. Also, we're

9:23

looking for Earth-like planets orbiting

9:26

M dwarf stars, which just means small

9:28

stars, and that makes it easier. So

9:31

go into a little bit why you're infatuated

9:33

with dwarf stars, M dwarf stars. Why are

9:35

they a good place to look? M

9:38

dwarfs are a great place to look for

9:40

a few reasons. These stars are about 10%

9:42

or 20% the size of the sun in

9:45

their diameter. So

9:48

when I was in high school and I was first getting

9:50

really interested in astronomy, I was told

9:52

a big lie, which is the sun

9:54

is an average star. And that's not

9:56

at all true. Most stars in the galaxy

9:58

are much, much smaller. and

10:00

less massive than the Sun, they are these M-dwarfs.

10:03

So one reason to go after these red dwarfs

10:05

or M-dwarfs is because of their size, it makes

10:07

the job easier. If we can detect them in

10:09

front of these small stars, then the signal is

10:11

much bigger because the planet

10:14

blocks more of the surface of the

10:16

star. The other one is that they're

10:18

much, much more numerous. So if you

10:20

draw a bubble around the Sun and

10:23

you go out to say, oh, I don't know, 30

10:26

light years or so, so that sounds really far

10:29

away, maybe that's close for an astronomer, that's just

10:31

the local neighbors, and you count up

10:33

all the stars in that bubble, there

10:35

are about 20 stars that are like the

10:37

Sun, and in that same

10:39

volume of space, there are 250 M-dwarfs. Then

10:44

almost certainly the closest planets to

10:46

us orbit those kind

10:49

of stars and not Sun-like stars.

10:51

I think there is some hint

10:53

that these, what we call terrestrial

10:55

Earth-like planets, have atmospheres, but actually

10:58

that's debated. We don't really

11:00

know for sure. There's not a consensus in the scientific

11:02

community. Day to

11:04

day, people are getting new data, analyzing

11:06

that data and going to conferences and debating about

11:08

whether they really have atmospheres. I

11:11

think we'll know the answer in a year or

11:13

two. So you're catching us at this really special

11:15

moment when we're just learning for the first time,

11:17

gosh, we now know for sure that there's rocky

11:19

planets out there and there's planets that

11:22

have the same temperature as the Earth, but do they

11:24

have atmospheres? We think that atmosphere is essential for life

11:26

as we know it. That's

11:28

what everybody's working on. It's a very exciting

11:31

moment in astronomy. You

11:33

know, I wonder about that phrase, life

11:35

is we know it. And I

11:37

get the impression that you're pretty liberal about

11:40

how far away from our kind of life you're

11:43

willing to go to find what

11:45

you could call life. Doesn't

11:47

have to be exactly what we know here. Is

11:50

that right? Yeah, when I'm thinking about

11:52

designing experiments to look for life in the

11:55

universe, we want to

11:57

cast the widest possible net. And

11:59

so. So, you know, one approach

12:01

is to look for signals from

12:03

intelligent civilizations, right, to what we

12:05

do, what we call SETI, looking

12:08

for radio signals or lasers

12:11

from other civilizations. And

12:13

that would be very, very similar to life on the Earth.

12:15

That would be tremendously exciting. What I'm

12:17

interested in is really stepping back and

12:19

saying, what are all the different paths,

12:21

all the different chemistries that we could

12:23

imagine that life would possibly take the

12:26

form of, and then thinking about

12:28

how do we design a set of experiments

12:30

generally to do that. I

12:36

was wondering about our own planet,

12:38

which for, I don't know, a couple

12:40

of billion years, as I understand it, there

12:43

was no oxygen involved. There was

12:45

life that was anaerobic. If

12:47

there hadn't been that transition

12:49

to aerobic life and

12:52

the anaerobic creatures

12:54

persisted, they might

12:56

have evolved into something more complex, no,

12:59

and interesting to us, although not at all like

13:01

us. I think one of the

13:03

most magical parts of astronomy is

13:05

the ability to travel through time. And

13:08

what I mean is when we look out into

13:10

the galaxy, we see all of these stars and

13:13

they all have planets, but

13:15

those stars have all been born

13:17

at different ages. So we can

13:19

find stars that are the same

13:21

age as the Sun. Okay,

13:24

there are piers, but we can find

13:26

stars that are recently born

13:29

and we can find stars that are much, much older than

13:31

the Sun. We can find stars that are 10 billion years

13:33

old. So they're twice as old as the Sun and the

13:35

Earth. And then

13:38

can you imagine doing the experiment

13:40

where now you're going to go

13:42

and study the atmospheres of all

13:44

of those planets and really trace

13:46

out, yeah, do most planets have

13:48

this pause, the sort of two

13:50

billion years where there might be

13:52

life on them, but it's anaerobic

13:54

and it's not producing oxygen. And

13:56

then at some point you have photosynthesis really

13:58

build up this critical mass. oxygen in the

14:01

atmosphere and then more complicated life forms

14:03

take place. And we could actually do

14:06

that experiment by finding planets around stars

14:08

of different ages. And

14:10

yes, it certainly looks like on the Earth, you

14:12

know, there was one set of organisms that worked

14:15

tirelessly for a very long time

14:17

until finally that oxygen was

14:20

built up. And that oxygen was probably toxic

14:22

to a lot of the organisms that

14:24

were present at the time. But then,

14:26

of course, other organisms moved into that

14:28

new space because

14:30

oxygen is great for in terms of how

14:33

organisms manipulate energy. And you could have multicellular

14:35

organisms and you could have larger plants and

14:37

animals and so on. So I think that

14:40

changes in the chemistry of the atmosphere do

14:42

very much affect the kind of organisms that

14:44

you get downstream. But the point is, as

14:46

astronomers, you could even imagine tracking that. And

14:49

I just that would be such an exciting

14:51

experiment to plan with powerful telescopes. What

14:54

do you think about the probability of

14:56

life throughout the universe? You can only see,

14:59

and you've only studied, a relatively small

15:02

part of it. Well, what gives you any

15:05

confidence that the place is full of

15:07

life? So here I

15:09

differ with a lot of my colleagues. I think

15:11

when I talk to other

15:13

astronomers around the world, most

15:16

of them seem to

15:19

be of the opinion that life

15:21

is present elsewhere in the universe.

15:24

But I'm not so sure. I am

15:27

more skeptical. I really need

15:29

to measure things to see that they're true.

15:31

Okay, that's how I come at science. So,

15:33

you know, the universe appears to be infinite.

15:35

And so there may be extremely distant galaxies,

15:38

and in those galaxies, there's planets with life.

15:40

But that's not a testable hypothesis. What I'm

15:42

really interested in figuring out is, you

15:45

know, within a few hundred light years of the Sun,

15:47

do those planets have life? Because, you know,

15:50

once you go and look at the closest

15:53

Earth-like planet outside the solar

15:55

system, right away, you have an important data point,

15:57

right? Maybe you get life all the time. 100%

16:00

of the time, life is extremely robust. And so you just

16:02

have to look at one other planet to know whether that's

16:04

true. And then you look at 10 of them

16:07

and you've learned, okay, well, it's less than

16:09

10% of the time. Or

16:11

you look at 100, it's less than 1% of the time. So

16:14

even just looking at a relatively small

16:16

number of planets would give us

16:18

a huge insight into

16:20

how improbable were the events that went down

16:22

on planet Earth 4 1,500,000,000 years ago. That's

16:26

why I'm really excited about that large telescope

16:28

that the community and NASA and everybody is

16:31

working towards. And so we have an understanding

16:33

of how big that telescope is, what

16:35

its capabilities need to be, and of course what the cost

16:37

would be. And that idea,

16:40

that's called the habitable world's observatory.

16:42

And so we think, we hope

16:44

that the next big mission that

16:47

NASA is going to work on,

16:49

okay, and start building and maybe

16:51

launch on a time scale of 10 or 15 years

16:54

is this facility. And it would

16:56

be a big telescope in space. It might

16:58

be about, oh, you know,

17:01

six meters across. So maybe about 20 feet

17:03

across, which is really big

17:05

for a space telescope. It would

17:07

have to be sensitive to specific wavelengths

17:09

and it would be looking for light

17:11

reflecting off of these Earth-like

17:13

planets and really allow us to look for

17:16

molecules such as oxygen. And

17:19

that would take considerable resources, which

17:22

prompts me to ask you a question

17:24

similar to the one we started with

17:26

about the usefulness of excitement and how

17:28

it propels things. I think

17:30

I've heard you say something roughly like, this

17:33

work that you're doing doesn't

17:35

solve problems here on Earth and

17:38

yet it attracts our wonder. What do you

17:40

suppose that is? What is that wonder and

17:42

excitement born of? Yeah,

17:45

I think that really gets to the heart

17:47

of why people

17:49

are drawn to science and drawn

17:51

to astronomy from

17:53

even the very youngest ages. You know, you

17:55

talk to five-year-old kids and

17:58

they're passionate about questions. And

18:00

then I go and I

18:02

talk to my dad and he's in his 80s and

18:04

he's asking the same questions. And

18:06

why is that? And it is

18:09

that sense of curiosity and wonder. And

18:11

I do think science is incredibly important

18:14

for coming up with practical

18:16

solutions for real problems

18:18

that we all face in

18:20

terms of technology, certainly

18:23

in terms of medicine, in terms

18:25

of really dealing with the scourge of disease

18:27

like cancer. Okay, that's a really important role

18:29

of science. But that's not the

18:32

only thing science does. There is this

18:34

other huge element of science which has

18:37

to do with just understanding who we

18:39

are as humans and our place in

18:42

the physical universe. And

18:45

I think the great contributions of

18:47

astronomy to human thought are

18:50

showing us that our lifetimes, if

18:53

you live decades, that your

18:56

lifetime is incredibly short compared to the

18:58

age of the universe. The

19:00

universe has been around for 14 billion years and

19:02

it looks like it will go on forever and

19:04

ever. And we're just here

19:06

for this little sliver of time. And

19:09

I think that gives us an enormous perspective.

19:11

The same is the physical size of the

19:13

universe, right? We've learned how big the universe

19:15

is and how small we are as people

19:17

in that space. And then

19:19

the study of life in the universe, I

19:21

think that also will very much put us

19:23

in perspective. Are we one of many, many

19:25

civilizations throughout the galaxy or are

19:27

we not? Are we really alone and unique in some

19:29

special way? And that's always

19:32

been the role of astronomy for hundreds of

19:34

years. And we just right now we're

19:36

live at this very special moment where

19:38

I think we're going to add

19:40

a very, very important part to that

19:42

puzzle, namely with how common life is

19:45

in the universe. Well

19:47

I'm eager to watch your progress as you find

19:50

the pieces to the puzzle. Thank

19:52

you for doing that puzzle work and

19:54

thank you for being with us today. I really

19:56

appreciate it. Oh, it's been my pleasure.

19:58

I just have absolutely. adored working

20:01

as an astronomer with all of the young

20:04

scientists and students over

20:06

several decades now. And I just can't

20:08

wait to see what they discover, what

20:10

we all get to discover in the

20:13

next couple of years. That's

20:15

great. Thank

20:17

you, David. When

20:22

we come back from our break, we'll find

20:24

out how a young girl growing up in

20:26

China during the Cultural Revolution was

20:28

inspired to become a neuroscientist while

20:31

correcting her father's English grammar. Our

20:37

program is sponsored by the

20:40

Kavli Prize, which honors scientists

20:42

for breakthroughs in astrophysics, nanoscience,

20:44

and neuroscience that transform

20:46

our understanding of the very big,

20:49

the very small, and the

20:51

very complex. From

20:53

scientific breakthroughs like the discovery

20:55

of CRISPR-Cas9 and the detection

20:57

of gravitational waves to

21:00

inventing new fields of research, Kavli

21:02

Prize Laureates pushed the limits of what

21:04

we know and advanced science in ways

21:07

that could not have been imagined. The

21:10

Kavli Prize is a partnership among

21:12

the Norwegian Academy of Science and

21:14

Letters, the Norwegian Ministry of Education

21:16

and Research, and the

21:18

U.S.-based Kavli Foundation in Los

21:20

Angeles, California. It's

21:23

one thing falling in love with a

21:25

house, picturing yourself moving in and calling

21:28

it home, and quite another navigating the

21:30

world of price negotiating, mortgage lenders, and

21:32

finding the budget that works best for

21:34

you. An agent who's a realtor can

21:36

make understanding that world easier. Realtors

21:39

have the expertise, access to proprietary data,

21:41

and tools to help you get from

21:43

imagining living somewhere to actually doing it.

21:46

That's the kind of help we can provide. Because

21:49

that's who we are. Realtors are members of

21:52

the National Association of Realtors. for

22:00

Convely Prizes. The prize

22:02

awarded to Doris Tseo cites

22:04

her work studying the cells in the

22:07

brain that are dedicated to identifying faces.

22:11

This is really interesting work you've done,

22:14

and congratulations on the prize. Thank you so

22:16

much. You were born in

22:18

China and spent a lot of your

22:20

youth in the library reading

22:22

all day long in the library. Why were

22:25

you in the library all day long? Oh,

22:27

it was because my parents simply didn't

22:29

have money to hire a babysitter. So

22:31

this was very cheap child care. And

22:35

your dad taught himself or

22:37

learned somehow about mathematics and

22:40

AI and various aspects of

22:42

science at night after finishing

22:45

logging all day long. Yeah,

22:47

he was really self-made. He knew

22:49

that the cultural revolution wouldn't last

22:51

forever, and he always dreamed

22:53

that he would be able to escape.

22:55

And when it was over, intellectual

22:58

pursuits would be valued again. And he

23:01

was just extremely curious about the world.

23:03

And it's amazing how much you can

23:06

learn, even when you're in the fourth

23:08

submenture with some books. I

23:10

was really interested to see what an

23:12

effect your father's intellectual life has had

23:14

on yours when your whole family was

23:16

in the US. And I think

23:19

you were at Cal Tech, and you

23:21

went on a camping trip, and

23:23

you were proofreading a paper of his

23:25

to help correct any mistakes in English.

23:28

What did you get from that paper, aside

23:30

from the fun of correcting your dad's grammar?

23:33

Yeah, it introduced me to this

23:35

idea that the

23:37

brain can compute

23:39

by somehow mirroring

23:42

what happens in the world. And that idea, you

23:44

know, at the time I was like a sophomore

23:46

in college, I felt like I was the only

23:48

person, me and my dad, we're the only person

23:51

who knew about this idea. And

23:53

then later on, I kind of learned that

23:55

many people have had this idea. It's actually

23:57

one dominant way of thinking about how the

23:59

brain works. It's very closely

24:01

related to this idea of predictive coding

24:03

that's actually used now to train chat

24:05

GPT. It's such a

24:07

powerful idea. But back then, I just

24:09

felt so happy. I

24:12

just discovered this in the course of correcting

24:14

my dad's English. Then

24:18

you took that idea further. I

24:21

think you were working with

24:23

Nancy Kanwisher, a fellow laureate,

24:25

and you were discovering face

24:27

patches, patches in the brain

24:29

that recognize elements of the face or the

24:31

whole face. How does that work? What's a

24:33

face patch? Yeah. So

24:35

a face patch is a

24:38

piece of cortex where

24:40

all the cells are dedicated to

24:43

processing faces. Indeed,

24:45

Nancy was the first to discover this.

24:48

She wrote this landmark paper in 1997. It's

24:51

the most cited paper of all time in

24:53

general neuroscience, where she scanned a

24:55

bunch of humans, put them in an fMRI scanner

24:57

and asked, is there any part of the brain

24:59

where there's more blood flow when you look at

25:01

pictures of faces than when you look at pictures

25:03

of other things? I just was

25:05

introduced the idea of face patches from her

25:08

paper. She actually reported that there were multiple

25:10

of these face hairs. There wasn't just one.

25:13

Her paper emphasized one of them because it

25:15

was the most reproducible across all the humans.

25:17

But actually, most of the people had multiple

25:19

of these face hairs. When we scanned monkeys,

25:22

we found that they had

25:24

six of these face patches. Each

25:26

patch is performing a different step and

25:28

they're working together to build a perception

25:31

of a face. So how many

25:33

aspects of the face are being checked out

25:35

to give you recognition? I've heard somewhere with

25:37

something like 50. You

25:41

can change a face in all these different

25:43

dimensions. What

25:45

we found was that the cells in these

25:47

face patches were sensitive to

25:49

change in these dimensions, meaning that

25:51

they would change their electrical firing

25:55

as you change the dimension. If you make

25:57

the eyes bigger in particular, over 70

25:59

percent, of the cells would start

26:02

firing more strongly. So we're extremely sensitive to the

26:04

size of the eyes. Sighs of the eye,

26:06

how far apart they are about that? Yeah, exactly

26:08

how far apart they are, how thick the

26:10

hair is. So there were so

26:12

many different dimensions that the cells seem to care

26:14

about. And I think it's actually more

26:16

than 50. But with 50

26:18

dimensions, it turns out that you can

26:20

produce a good likeness of anyone's face.

26:23

Like I can describe

26:25

your face, Alan, with just

26:27

50 dimensions. And it's not going

26:29

to look perfect. It's not going to capture all the details.

26:31

But I'll be able to recognize it. And

26:34

the cells definitely are able to code all 50

26:36

of these dimensions. And we think more. So it's

26:38

a very high dimensional space that the cells

26:40

are coding. I've read a couple of

26:43

years ago that a single cell

26:45

in the brain could recognize the

26:47

face. Is that true? I

26:50

think you're referring to this famous

26:53

Jennifer Aniston cell. This

26:55

was a cell that responded to

26:57

the picture of Jennifer Aniston, but

26:59

not to picture Brad Pitt. And

27:02

so this was reported to be

27:04

a cell that was

27:06

just responsible for representing her. I

27:10

think the cell was actually the most

27:12

misunderstood cell in all of neuroscience, because

27:14

it's such a famous neuron. It's probably

27:16

the most famous neuron ever.

27:19

In fact, one colleague has joked that

27:21

he probably has a cell in his

27:23

brain for representing the Jennifer Aniston cell.

27:27

But the reason I

27:29

say it's misunderstood is that if you

27:32

look in the supplementary information for that

27:34

paper, it turns out that that cell

27:36

also responded to the picture of Lisa

27:38

Kudrow, who is another

27:40

actor on Friends. So I

27:42

think the corrected interpretation of that cell is

27:45

that it wasn't just coding this

27:47

one person, Jennifer Aniston, but it was coding

27:49

this concept of Friends. And just

27:51

like all of memory, it's part

27:54

of an associative network. I don't

27:56

believe there's any cell in the

27:58

brain that's only responsible. to

28:01

one single person space. So in

28:03

a way, there's a network of

28:05

cells that have developed in

28:07

response to aspects of friends. Yes.

28:10

And Jennifer Aniston is an important part of that.

28:13

Yeah, that's very interesting because that sort of throws

28:15

light on what I wanted to ask

28:18

you about is, how does the code

28:20

work? How do these various

28:23

cells in different parts of the brain, and

28:25

from one patch to another, how

28:28

do they cooperate to give you a

28:30

face that you can remember? Yeah,

28:32

I mean, that's a huge question. I can't,

28:35

we certainly haven't solved that question. How

28:37

exactly do they cooperate? So

28:39

we have hypothesis about what they're

28:41

doing. For example, as you

28:44

go anterior in the brain, as you go

28:46

closer from the back to the front of

28:48

the brain, the processing

28:50

becomes more abstract and complex. So

28:53

the back of your brain is like where your early visual cortex is, and

28:56

the very front of your brain is your frontal

28:58

lobe that's responsible for extremely high-level decision making. So

29:01

anyways, within this face patch system, as you move

29:03

anterior, what you find is that the

29:06

different patches, their processing becomes more

29:09

and more abstract. So in the

29:11

most posterior patch, the cells care about specific

29:13

features like the eye, and

29:16

then as you go anterior, in the

29:18

most anterior patch, they actually can respond to

29:20

the head orientation. So a face can be

29:22

looking straight at you or looking up or

29:24

looking to the sideways, and the cell will

29:26

respond the same way. And

29:28

we think that's one way that the cells are

29:31

building up this perceptive face. So in an earlier

29:34

area, you might have eight different cells for

29:37

representing the eight different views of a person, and

29:39

then in the most anterior area, those eight

29:41

cells converge on a single cell that represents

29:44

that person invariant to how they're looking. This

29:46

may be a question more

29:48

for Nancy Kanwisher. It's my

29:50

own personal response to

29:53

faces. I have

29:55

prosopagnosia, which is

29:57

face blindness. When I tell people I... I

30:00

could have had dinner with somebody the

30:02

night before and meet them the next day, and

30:05

I don't know who they are. And

30:08

then as I apologize, I say, I'm

30:10

really sorry I have face blindness. And

30:12

they say something like, oh yeah, I have that too. Not

30:16

knowing that it's a real condition. What's

30:18

gone wrong in my brain? Yeah,

30:21

so there's different types of

30:23

prasipagnosia. If you are

30:26

fortunate enough to have a lesion and suffer

30:28

a stroke in your temporal lobe and just

30:30

lose your face area,

30:32

then you definitely will become prasipagnosic. But

30:35

in your case, obviously, that's not the

30:37

case. So there's also this syndrome called

30:41

developmental prasipagnosia. So

30:44

we'd have to put you in fMRI scanner to know

30:47

for sure. But I find

30:49

it very interesting that you're prasipagnosic because I've

30:51

read that Brad Pitt is also prasipagnosic. And

30:54

yeah, I wonder if that you're

30:56

both actors. Maybe there's a higher

30:58

percentage among actors. You wonder where you

31:00

think there is. It would make sense to me.

31:03

Why, that's interesting, why is that? Because

31:05

maybe you don't feel this fear, I

31:07

don't know. Like normally

31:09

people react in a certain way to other

31:12

people because of just these natural social signals.

31:14

An actor, I imagine, I don't know, you

31:16

have to overcome those innate responses and maybe

31:18

it helps to not have

31:21

that recognition. Maybe I ought

31:23

to take advantage of that intuition of

31:25

yours because kind of the opposite

31:27

happens with me. And I

31:29

think it was happening long

31:32

before I realized there was such a thing

31:34

as face blindness. I

31:36

would be very anxious in a

31:39

social situation because I didn't

31:41

know if the person I was talking to was

31:43

somebody I shouldn't really recognize. Or

31:47

if they just looked vaguely familiar, had a type

31:49

of face that was similar to somebody I knew,

31:51

but I wouldn't even go that far. That'd just

31:53

be a blank to me. And

31:56

so I'd get nervous, anxious,

31:58

just talking with somebody. I thought

32:00

I might be meeting for the first time or maybe

32:02

not. But maybe I'll tell, maybe I can

32:04

take advantage of what you said before about actors

32:08

in kind of a protective mode.

32:11

Yeah, no, I just read a story

32:13

about someone who was completely passive-pagnosic and,

32:16

you know, he described how he got a

32:18

job working at the Oscars because he just

32:20

wasn't fazed when he saw these famous people.

32:23

He wasn't fazed, that's funny. Maybe

32:27

that's my next career. Is

32:33

it that the brain has

32:35

specialized areas for recognizing

32:37

faces, but not

32:39

for other objects? Or do

32:42

classes of objects have

32:44

their own circuits the same

32:47

way faces do? Yeah,

32:49

it turns out that faces are not that

32:51

special in this respect and there are multiple

32:53

other networks for representing other

32:56

regions of object space. It

32:58

turns out that there's a

33:00

region for representing stubby

33:02

things like a box or a USB stick

33:04

or a radio. So it's not a single

33:06

category, but it's a single shape. And

33:09

then there's another region that's representing spiky

33:11

things like chairs and helicopters and spiders.

33:14

And so those other networks

33:16

are not specialized for a specific meaningful category

33:18

the way the face area is, but

33:20

they are specialized for representing a

33:23

specific class of shape. And

33:25

they follow exactly the same principles as

33:28

the space patch network. So you have

33:30

increasing invariance as you go anterior and

33:33

they use the same type of code. And

33:36

so actually it was, you know,

33:39

when I first started working on the space

33:41

patch system, people would say, you know, that's

33:43

a total unicorn. You're not going to learn

33:45

anything general about object representation from setting face

33:47

areas. And I

33:50

think we've actually shown that that's not

33:52

true at all. And the face patch system

33:54

has actually been like a Rosetta stone for

33:56

figuring out how all types of objects are

33:58

represented. And it turns out it's. it

34:00

generalizes extraordinarily,

34:03

including the rest of object space. So

34:05

as the person develops from

34:08

an infant on up, they're learning, obviously they're

34:10

learning a lot of words, but they're also

34:12

learning a lot of objects, it sounds like

34:15

you're saying? Yeah, exactly.

34:17

You gain so much experience

34:19

with different objects. I

34:22

should say this insight about

34:25

the brain organizing objects

34:28

into this space, this object

34:30

space, really comes from recent

34:32

advances in AI. So people have these AI

34:35

systems that also know nothing about objects, and

34:37

just show them lots of objects. You

34:40

ask the network to either categorize the

34:42

objects, or you ask the network to

34:46

organize the objects such that objects that

34:48

are similar or close together. These

34:51

networks develop these representations and you can look in

34:53

them under the hood because you built this network

34:55

so you can look at every single unit with

34:57

selected form and turns out that

34:59

they're representing the objects in this orderly

35:01

space and that's how we got this

35:03

hint that the primate brain might

35:06

be doing the same thing and that's

35:08

how we found it. So yes, just through

35:10

exposure to different objects, you can learn to

35:13

build these maps. It almost seems like

35:15

an inevitable consequence of

35:19

doing object recognition. You

35:22

make me think of my ability to picture

35:24

a fire plug, and I

35:26

can picture it in different ways with

35:28

connectors to fire hoses in different parts

35:30

of the fire plug. I

35:32

have these parts in my brain that I can

35:34

put together, but whenever I see a fire plug,

35:36

it doesn't have to be like the last one

35:39

I saw. There's a generic

35:41

fire plug shape made of

35:43

constituent parts that let me identify it

35:46

as what it really is. But it

35:49

must be tantalizing to you

35:51

to figure out, try to figure out how

35:54

those parts come together. It

35:56

seems to me, here's my real question. It

35:59

seems to me, that you can't have pictures

36:01

of objects stored in your brain,

36:03

because that would mean if you

36:05

saw an object that was slightly

36:07

different, you wouldn't recognize it. You

36:10

have to be able to draw on

36:12

parts the same way you do with

36:14

faces. Wow. That is so profound. Exactly.

36:17

That is what we're after. We want

36:19

to understand how these parts are represented.

36:21

And that's one of the reasons we're

36:24

mucking around now a totally new part of the

36:26

brain, far away from the face patches, because

36:28

I think there are earlier processes that

36:30

are actually building 3D models

36:33

of the world around us. And they

36:35

work more like Legos. They're really building

36:38

these different objects from parts.

36:40

And you can build any object that you

36:42

want. And the way you represent

36:44

it as an object is based on a totally

36:46

different set of principles, based on topology and how

36:48

it's connected. And this is actually... I

36:51

wrote a paper about this with my father recently. Tell

36:54

me about that paper, because that brings us full

36:56

circle. We started off with

36:58

you studying your father's paper that got you

37:00

really motivated a lot into this field. And

37:03

now, what's the paper you're working on that

37:06

has to do with this topology? Yeah,

37:08

the title of paper is a

37:10

topological solution to object segmentation and

37:12

tracking. And it's

37:15

directly addressing the question that you just

37:17

raised. How do you put together

37:20

parts into holes? And

37:23

how do you do this flexibly so you can

37:25

create any part? And so, our

37:29

paper is addressing how the visual system

37:31

can take all these pixels from our

37:33

retina where they're completely not organized into

37:35

any structure, and how the

37:37

visual system can then organize it into

37:39

discrete objects. And

37:43

the answer that we come to is

37:45

that we have to take

37:47

advantage of the fact that we're living in a 3D world,

37:49

and so we can see things from different perspectives. And

37:52

based on how those perspectives are

37:54

related, we show mathematically

37:56

how to solve this problem of

37:58

dividing the world into... different

38:00

objects. So that's

38:02

what the paper is about. It's

38:04

a mathematical theory of how you solve

38:06

this problem of dividing pixels

38:09

into objects and also how you track

38:11

those objects as they move around. It

38:15

was my pandemic project. Well,

38:19

you used the pandemic well. My

38:22

dad and I were discussing these ideas for over

38:24

a decade, but we never somehow got to the

38:26

stage of being able to write it up. We

38:30

need to actually show that it works. So

38:32

I actually programmed it. I showed that the

38:35

system worked and then we

38:37

wrote it, we dealt with reviewers, we

38:39

got it published. That's

38:42

wonderful. What a wonderful story. What

38:44

a terrific conversation this has been

38:46

too. I thank you so

38:48

much. I know you're very busy and I really appreciate

38:50

you taking the time to talk with me. Thank

38:53

you. I really enjoyed this. This

39:03

program is sponsored by the

39:05

Kavli Prize, which honors scientists

39:07

for breakthroughs in astrophysics, nanoscience,

39:09

and neuroscience. The

39:11

Kavli Prize is a partnership among

39:13

the Norwegian Academy of Science and

39:15

Letters, the Norwegian Ministry of Education

39:17

and Research, and the

39:19

US-based Kavli Foundation in Los

39:22

Angeles, California. David

39:25

Charbonneau is professor of astrophysics in

39:27

the Department of Astronomy at Harvard

39:29

University. He recently

39:32

co-chaired the National Academy's Exoplanet

39:34

Science Strategy. He

39:36

shares the 2024 Kavli Prize for astrophysics

39:40

with Sarah Seger. Doris

39:43

Sow is professor of neurobiology at

39:45

the University of California, Berkeley. She

39:48

shares this year's Kavli Prize in

39:51

neuroscience with Nancy Kanwisher and

39:53

Vinrich Freiburg. This

39:56

episode was edited and produced by

39:58

our executive producer Graham. shed

40:01

with help from our associate producer

40:03

Jean Chumet. Our publicist

40:05

is Sarah Hill. Our

40:07

researcher is Elizabeth Ohini, and

40:09

the sound engineer is Erica Huang. The

40:12

music is courtesy of the Stefan-Kernig

40:15

Trio. Next

40:24

week, my guests will be two more 2024

40:27

Cauvery Prize Laureates, both

40:29

for nanoscience. Chad

40:31

Merkin pioneered the use of microscopic carriers

40:34

that are able to deliver new types

40:36

of medicines. I think of

40:38

the big opportunity is being able to build drugs

40:40

that are much bigger than the normal

40:42

drugs that the pharmaceutical industry works with, that

40:45

have a type of multifunctionality that allow

40:47

you to solve major challenges in terms

40:49

of treating disease. And that's why I'm

40:51

very bullish on the idea that not

40:53

just our work, but much of the

40:55

work in nanomedicine is going to lead

40:57

to treatments for disease where we need

40:59

really powerful treatments and ways of giving

41:01

a lot of people hope that don't

41:03

have much hope. Robert

41:06

Langer also made breakthroughs in drug

41:08

delivery by devising ways for

41:10

drugs to be released in the body

41:12

slowly and over time. He

41:14

also pioneered the development of lab-grown

41:17

human tissues and organs. Now

41:19

you can make skin for

41:21

burn victims and patients that have diabetic

41:23

skin ulcers. One of my

41:26

students has a company that's making new blood

41:28

vessels that probably will get approved by

41:30

the FDA, at least I hope so very soon.

41:33

But it's already being used to help people

41:35

in Ukraine who've been badly wounded. They're

41:38

making new blood vessels for them. And then

41:40

there's others at earlier stages. In fact, we've

41:42

worked with Steve Zytels, who's

41:45

a surgeon for Julie Andrews, and

41:47

making new vocal chords. Both

41:50

Merkin and Langer have played major

41:52

roles in developing the biotechnology industry.

41:56

Between them, they've filed thousands of patents

41:58

and helped found multiple companies. They

42:01

share the 2024 Nano Science

42:03

Cauvery Prize with Paul Alivisatos.

42:07

For more details about Clear and Vivid and

42:09

to sign up for my newsletter, please

42:11

visit alanalda.com. And

42:14

you can also find us on Facebook and

42:16

Instagram at Clear and Vivid. Thanks

42:19

for listening. Bye-bye. Is

42:30

this house a good price compared to others in the

42:32

area? Are prices going up or down? If

42:34

I don't make an offer right this very moment, will

42:36

I miss my chance? These are just some of the

42:38

questions a home buyer might ask. And these are the

42:40

sorts of questions an agent who is a Realtor can

42:42

help answer. Because Realtors have the

42:45

expertise, data, and access to specialty training to

42:47

help you navigate the process of buying a

42:49

home. They provide support, guidance, and have your

42:51

back every step of the way. That's what

42:53

Realtors do. Because that's who we

42:55

are. Realtors are members of the

42:57

National Association of Realtors. Thank

43:00

you.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features