Podchaser Logo
Home
Innovation 2.0: The Influence You Have

Innovation 2.0: The Influence You Have

Released Monday, 13th May 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Innovation 2.0: The Influence You Have

Innovation 2.0: The Influence You Have

Innovation 2.0: The Influence You Have

Innovation 2.0: The Influence You Have

Monday, 13th May 2024
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

This is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedanta.

0:03

Philip Zimbardo grew up poor in New York

0:06

City, in the South Bronx. As

0:08

he went to school and played in his neighborhood,

0:11

he noticed something. There were lots of

0:13

ways for kids from poor families to

0:15

get into trouble. One of the

0:17

things about growing up poor is you're surrounded

0:19

by evil, meaning people whose job

0:22

it is to get good kids to do bad things

0:24

for money. And even as a

0:26

little kid, I was always curious about why some kids

0:28

got seduced and other kids like me were able to

0:30

resist. Were some kids

0:32

smarter? Tougher? Lots

0:35

of people might draw such conclusions, but from an

0:37

early age, Phil found

0:39

himself interested in another explanation.

0:42

The context in which a good kid would

0:45

do something bad. The situation.

0:49

At school, Phil

0:52

got close to a classmate who was interested

0:54

in the same questions. And it was a

0:57

little Jewish kid named Stanley Milgram. We

0:59

were in the same class. We sat side by side.

1:01

He was the smartest kid in the class. He

1:04

won all the medals at graduation, so obviously nobody

1:06

liked him because we were all envious of him.

1:08

But he was super smart and super serious.

1:14

If you know anything about psychology, you

1:16

will know that these teenagers went on

1:19

to become two of the most influential

1:21

psychologists in history. Phil

1:24

became famous for conducting the Stanford

1:26

Prison Experiment where he turned the

1:28

university's psychology department into a makeshift

1:30

prison. Stanley Milgram

1:33

made his mark with a study that

1:35

examined the power of situations to seduce

1:37

good people to do bad things. It

1:40

involved asking a volunteer to administer a

1:42

memory test to another person. If

1:45

the answers were wrong, the volunteer

1:47

was told to deliver a series

1:49

of electrical shocks as punishment.

1:53

The study has invited a great deal of

1:55

admiration and a great deal of criticism over

1:57

the years. We're

2:00

going to begin today's show by taking you

2:02

through this famous experiment. As

2:04

you listen, pay attention to how you're

2:06

responding to the scene that unfolds, what you

2:09

think about the different characters, and how you

2:11

relate to them. Once

2:13

it's done, we're going to talk

2:15

with a psychologist who realized that

2:17

most people overlook something in the

2:19

experiment. We so often sort of

2:21

simulate, if I was in that Milgram shock

2:24

experiment, what would I do if I was

2:26

the study participant, right? Would I actually stand

2:28

up and go against these directives and say

2:30

no? But we kind of flipped

2:32

that idea on its head. Today

2:38

on the show, we continue our Innovation 2.0

2:41

series by looking

2:43

at the underappreciated power we exert

2:45

on others and how this

2:47

knowledge can transform our relationships, both at

2:49

work and in our personal lives. Flipping

2:54

the script, this week on

2:56

Hidden Brain. Support

3:05

for Hidden Brain comes from Discover. We're

3:08

24-7 US-based live customer service from

3:10

Discover. Everyone has the

3:12

option to talk to a real person anytime,

3:14

day or night. Yes, you heard that

3:16

right. You can talk to a human

3:18

on the Discover customer service team anytime. So the

3:20

next time you have a question about your

3:23

credit card, call 1-800-Discover to get the

3:25

service you deserve. Limitations apply.

3:29

See terms at

3:31

discover.com-slash-creditcard. Support

3:38

for Hidden Brain comes from Alta Beauty. This AAPI

3:40

Heritage Month, Alta Beauty is

3:42

celebrating the joy of belonging.

3:44

Belonging to a community composed

3:46

of intricate connections. Belonging

3:49

to the heritage and birthright that is beauty.

3:53

Alta Beauty spotlights the AAPI community, passing

3:56

the mic to brand founders and creators to

3:58

tell their stories sent to them. centered

4:00

on heritage, joy, and beauty. Shop

4:03

AAPI owned and founded brands

4:05

at Ulta Beauty Stores and

4:07

ulta.com. Stanley

4:15

Milgram grew up in a world that

4:17

seemed bent on destroying itself. World

4:21

War II was raging in Europe and Asia, and by

4:23

the time he was eight, the U.S. was

4:25

swept up in the conflict. 1941,

4:27

a date which will live in infamy. The

4:44

fields of battle were far from Stanley's home,

4:47

but as he grew older, he couldn't stop

4:49

thinking about the war and its implications. Stanley

4:53

was consumed by some big questions. Why

4:56

did so many people willingly kill Jews in

4:58

the Holocaust? Was everyone

5:00

who followed Nazi orders inherently evil?

5:03

Here he is in an educational film. Phil

5:17

Zimbato remembers his classmate asking those

5:19

same questions at James Monroe High

5:21

School. The high school student

5:24

was worried that the Holocaust could happen again

5:26

in America. And everybody said, Stanley, that was

5:28

Nazi Germany, that was then, we're not that

5:30

kind of people. And he would

5:32

say, I'll bet they thought the same thing. And

5:35

at the bottom line, he says, how do you know how

5:37

you would act unless you're in the situation? How

5:40

do you know how you would act unless you're

5:43

in the situation? Stanley's

5:45

theory was that the context that

5:47

people found themselves in shaped their

5:49

behavior. This went for

5:51

Nazis, but it went for ordinary people too. Most

5:55

of us never get to find out if we will

5:57

behave like Nazis, because most of us

5:59

never find us themselves in situations where

6:02

we are asked to behave like Nazis. By

6:07

the early 1960s, as a psychology professor

6:09

at Yale, Stanley decided to

6:11

test this idea. Under what

6:13

conditions would a person obey authority who

6:16

commanded actions that went against conscience? These

6:18

are exactly the questions that I wanted

6:20

to investigate at Yale University. Stanley

6:23

wanted to put volunteers in a situation where

6:25

they would be asked to do something that

6:27

was clearly wrong. Would they do

6:29

it? Follow instructions?

6:32

Obey orders? He

6:35

came up with a scenario that was

6:37

simple, ingenious, and wildly

6:39

controversial. An experimenter

6:42

wearing a lab coat invited volunteers

6:44

into a room. The

6:46

volunteers were told they were part of

6:48

a study about learning and memory. Some

6:51

would play the role of teacher, while others

6:53

would play a student. What

6:56

you are going to hear next is a

6:58

recreation of the study using voice actors. The

7:01

dialogue is drawn from a 1962 documentary

7:04

that describes the experiment. Before

7:08

we begin, we should know that some listeners

7:10

may find this section upsetting because

7:12

it involves descriptions of someone inflicting

7:14

pain on another person. Also,

7:17

there are two liberties we have taken in this

7:20

recreation. First, in the real

7:22

version of this experiment, the student

7:24

responded to the teacher's questions by

7:26

silently flipping a switch. We

7:29

have given voice to those actions. Second,

7:33

we have imagined the internal monologues of

7:35

some of the people in the experiment.

7:38

Those inner voices sound different from the

7:40

things they say aloud, and

7:42

you will hear them both throughout the scene. So

7:45

that is the setup for the experiment.

7:48

Remember, there was an experimenter and two

7:50

volunteers, one playing the role of

7:52

teacher and the other playing the role of student. The

7:55

experimenter began by explaining the

7:57

purpose of the memory test. find

8:00

out just what effect different people have on

8:02

each other as teachers and learners and also

8:04

what effect punishment will have on learning in

8:07

this situation. The experimenter told

8:09

the person playing the role of student to

8:11

sit in a chair. The

8:13

experimenter strapped down the student's arms

8:16

and attached an electrode to his wrist.

8:19

The electrode, the student was told, was

8:21

connected to a shock generator.

8:24

Then the experimenter explained, the teacher will read

8:26

a list of word pairs to you like

8:29

these, blue, girl, nice

8:31

day, fat neck and

8:33

so forth. You are to try to remember

8:35

each pair. For the next time through, the

8:37

teacher will read only the first word or

8:39

the first half of the word pair. The

8:42

student was asked to remember the second half

8:44

of the word pair. The experimenter

8:46

made sure to ask. Do you have any questions now

8:48

before we go to the next room? No,

8:51

but I think I should say this. About

8:53

two years ago, I was at the Veterans

8:55

Hospital in West Haven and while there they

8:57

detected a heart condition. There's nothing

8:59

serious, but as long as I'm having these

9:01

shocks, how strong

9:03

are they? How dangerous are they?

9:06

Well, no, although they may be painful, they're

9:08

not dangerous. Next,

9:10

the experimenter ushered the volunteer playing

9:12

the role of teacher into another

9:14

room. He gave him

9:16

a set of instructions. You will read each

9:19

pair of words in this list once to

9:21

the learner until you've read the entire list.

9:23

Direct your voice toward the microphone as the

9:25

rooms are only partly soundproof. Now,

9:28

if he gives the correct answer, you say

9:30

correct and go on to the next line.

9:32

The correct answer is

9:34

indicated in the right margin. I see. All

9:36

right. Looks easy enough. The experiment got on

9:39

the way. Attention learner, your teacher

9:41

is about to begin the test. Try

9:43

and remember the word pairs. Ready?

9:46

Begin. Blue. Girl. Right

9:48

so far. Nice. I think day. Fat.

9:51

The person in the other room made a mistake.

9:53

Fat. Was it hat? No. Wet.

9:56

The volunteer playing the role of teacher will

9:58

tell the learner that he was... was wrong.

10:01

As punishment, he would administer

10:03

an electric jolt. Incorrect. You'll

10:05

now get a shock of 75 volts. Oh!

10:10

Seems nervous. The experimenter

10:13

in the lab coat, meanwhile, was observing

10:15

the process. Please continue.

10:18

Cool. Okay, I'm pretty sure it's

10:20

day. Wrong. It's head.

10:22

105 volts. Ow! Come

10:24

on, get it right. I don't want to shock

10:26

you. Future, please continue. What should I do? You gotta

10:28

keep going. I wonder how far it'll go. Wrong. 135

10:30

volts. Answer. The

10:33

students trapped at the chair in the other

10:35

room kept making the day. Wrong.

10:37

Each time, the experimenter urged the volunteer

10:39

playing the role of teacher to keep

10:42

going, to administer a

10:44

stronger jolt of electricity. Wrong.

10:46

It's harsh. 150 volts. Ow!

10:49

Experimenter, get me out of here. That's all. He wants to

10:51

quit. Get me out of here. I told

10:53

you I had heart trouble. My heart is starting to bother

10:55

me now. Get me out of

10:57

here, please. My heart is starting to bother

11:00

me. I refuse to go on. Let me

11:02

out. He refused to go on. You want

11:04

me to keep going? The experiment requires you

11:06

to continue, teacher. Please continue. The next word

11:08

is sad. Hi,

11:11

hello. Next word's sad.

11:14

Uh, I don't know. Day? Wrong.

11:18

I'm up to 180 volts. 180. You're

11:20

gonna stop. Please continue. As the

11:22

shocks increased, so did the pain.

11:25

And so did the protests coming from the next

11:27

room. He's got a heart condition in there. I'm

11:30

gonna go. Your choice. Please continue.

11:33

Now I got a shock. 180 volts.

11:36

Ow! I can't stand the pain. You can't stand

11:38

it. Let me out of here. So,

11:40

are you gonna keep shocking him? You hear

11:42

him hollering. Poor guy. He's in there screaming.

11:44

I said before, the shocks may be painful,

11:46

but they're not dangerous. But he's in there

11:48

hollering. So, stop. You can't stand it.

11:50

What if something happens? You don't have to keep going.

11:54

The volunteer being asked to administer electric shocks

11:56

is in a difficult position. The

11:59

experimenter is O.J. him to continue, even

12:01

as the person in the next room begs

12:04

to be spared. Should he

12:06

keep going or stop? The

12:08

experiment requires that you continue, teacher. Whether

12:10

the learner likes it or not, we

12:12

must go on until he's learned all

12:14

the word pairs. I refuse to take

12:16

responsibility of him getting hurt. I mean,

12:19

he's there calling. It's absolutely essential that

12:21

you continue, teacher. There is too many

12:23

of them left. Please, go on. Who's

12:25

going to take the responsibility if anything

12:27

happens to the gentleman? I'm responsible for

12:29

anything that happens here. Please

12:31

continue. As the experiment progressed, the memory

12:34

test became more demanding. Next one.

12:37

Slow, walk, dance, truck, music.

12:40

Come on. Answer please. Come

12:42

on, get this right. I know it's for science, but I don't want to

12:44

hurt you. Wrong. 195 bucks. Ow!

12:47

Let me out of here! Continue, please. Let me out

12:50

of here! I don't know. You have no

12:52

right to keep... The experiment requires... I know it does, sir, but I

12:54

mean, you do know what he's getting in for. He's up to 195

12:56

bucks. That's pretty high. After

13:01

the study reached about 330 volts,

13:04

the screams from the next room were

13:06

unhealed. If

13:10

the learner doesn't answer in a

13:12

reasonable time, about four or five

13:14

seconds, consider the answer wrong. And

13:17

follow the same procedures you had been

13:19

doing for wrong answers. Say, wrong, tell

13:22

them the number of volts, give him

13:24

the punishment. Go

13:27

on, please, with the experiment. Please continue.

13:29

He's freaking... Soft,

13:32

rug, pillow, hair,

13:35

grass. Answer, please. Go

13:39

on, teacher. 360 volts. I

13:43

think something's happened to that fellow in there. I didn't

13:45

get no answer. He was hollering

13:48

at less voltage. Can you check to see if

13:50

he's all right, please? Not once we've started. Please

13:53

continue, teacher. In

13:55

all, Stanley Milgram ran about 20 different iterations

13:57

of this study over a span of years.

14:00

of several years. In

14:02

this version, many of the volunteers

14:04

playing the role of teacher showed

14:06

discomfort but continued with the experiment.

14:09

More than half went all the way to 450 vols,

14:13

even when the screams from the next room went

14:15

silent and the student was

14:17

presumably unconscious. Why

14:19

didn't the volunteer stop? Stanley

14:22

later debriefs some of the volunteers. Why didn't

14:24

you stop anyway? I did stop but he

14:26

kept going, keep going. But

14:29

why didn't you just disregard what he said? He

14:32

says it's got to go on, the experiment. If

14:35

you're familiar with the study, you already know

14:37

that the student in the other room was

14:39

an actor and not actually

14:41

given electric shanks. The

14:44

screams and cries of protest were

14:46

carefully timed recordings. The

14:49

only target of the experiment were the volunteers

14:51

who played the role of teacher, the

14:54

people who had to administer the shanks.

15:00

Stanley Milgram's study generated enormous

15:02

attention and controversy. Admires

15:05

drew parallels between the experiment and

15:07

what happened in Nazi Germany. They

15:09

said, look, people are sheep. They

15:12

can be easily misled by demagogues

15:14

and dictators. Critics

15:17

of the study said, no, those

15:19

conclusions are vastly exaggerated.

15:22

They questioned whether the volunteers actually behave

15:24

the way the experiment suggested. Some

15:27

critics said that many volunteers simply

15:29

refused to go along. Beyond

15:34

the academic debates, the study prompted

15:36

an entire sub-genre of books and

15:38

movies. Even today,

15:40

people find the study fascinating and

15:43

they find it fascinating for one reason. How,

15:45

they ask, could people who know that

15:48

something is wrong go along with it?

15:51

Are such people typical? Is

15:53

everyone susceptible to such influence? Am

15:55

I? As

16:00

we listen to the details of the study, we

16:02

can't help but ask, what would I

16:05

do? Would I follow orders

16:07

and zap the person screaming in the other room?

16:17

But Vanessa Bonds, a psychologist at

16:19

Cornell University, realized there was

16:21

something no one was paying attention to. Everyone

16:25

was asking what was going on in the

16:27

minds of the volunteers and how difficult the

16:29

situation was for them. No

16:42

one was asking whether it was difficult

16:44

for the experimenter wearing the lab coat

16:47

to tell the volunteers to administer electric

16:49

shocks. To the extent

16:51

we think of the experimenter at all, we

16:54

might imagine someone who enjoyed putting people

16:56

in difficult situations, a sort of mad

16:58

scientist. Vanessa

17:00

asked a deceptively simple question.

17:08

Vanessa's insight was radical. What

17:11

if you looked at the experiment, not from the point

17:13

of view of the students screaming in the next room,

17:15

and not from the point of view of the

17:18

volunteer administering the shocks, but

17:20

from the point of view of the person giving the

17:22

instructions? Teacher, please continue.

17:29

What if you treated the experimenter as

17:31

the object of study? Get

17:33

me out of here. I told you I

17:35

had heart trouble. My heart is starting to

17:37

bother me now. Get me out of here,

17:40

please. My heart is starting to bother me.

17:42

The experiment requires you to continue, teacher. Please

17:44

continue. Please continue. I

17:47

said before, the shocks may be painful,

17:50

but they're not. So

17:52

are you going to keep on talking? Why

18:02

do so few of us put ourselves in the

18:04

shoes of the experimenter? Why

18:07

don't we ask how difficult it was for him to

18:09

issue those instructions? Why

18:12

is it, when we hear the story, we

18:14

automatically put ourselves in the shoes of

18:16

the volunteers, the people receiving

18:18

the instructions? Vanessa

18:21

realized that we all naturally gravitate to

18:23

the point of view of the volunteers

18:25

and not the point of view of

18:27

the experimenter because we all

18:29

instinctively know what it feels like to

18:31

have other people put us in uncomfortable

18:33

situations. We think

18:36

of our bosses, our partners, our co-workers,

18:38

and how they affect our lives and

18:40

change our moods. We

18:42

think of the aggressive driver next to us or

18:45

the other patrons at the restaurant who are

18:47

so loud and obnoxious that they ruin

18:49

our meal. We feel

18:51

buffeted and pushed and pulled by those around

18:54

us. So one

18:56

thing we don't ask, what

18:58

effect do I have on other

19:00

people? There's been a

19:03

long history of research on social influence and persuasion,

19:05

so we do know a lot about how

19:08

other people influence us, but we don't

19:10

know so much about how we experience

19:12

our influence of other people. When

19:16

we are intensely focused on how the world affects

19:18

us and not how we affect

19:20

the world, this can

19:22

have profound consequences for

19:24

both good and evil. You're

19:34

listening to Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedantam.

19:41

Support for Hidden Brain comes from T-Mobile. The

19:44

most innovative companies are going further

19:46

with T-Mobile for business. The

19:50

PGA of America is helping lower

19:52

scores and elevate fan experiences with

19:54

AI coaching tools and 5G connected

19:57

cameras. AAA is

19:59

getting more drivers. back on the

20:01

road fast with location telematics. And

20:04

the Las Vegas Grand Prix is

20:06

powering race day operations with 5G

20:08

connectivity giving fans an experience

20:11

at the speed they deserve. This

20:13

is accelerating innovation with T-Mobile

20:15

for Business. Take

20:17

your business further at t-mobile.com

20:20

slash now. This

20:33

is Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedanta. When

20:35

Vanessa Bonds was a graduate student at

20:38

Columbia University, she worked on a study.

20:41

Every day she would leave her

20:43

apartment in the Morningside Heights neighborhood

20:45

and take the subway from 116th

20:47

Street to Penn Station. Penn

20:50

Station. Once she

20:52

was there, she had to do something she

20:54

found very difficult. Basically I would just

20:56

go up to random people in Penn Station, say,

20:58

hey we've got this questionnaire. Vanessa

21:01

no longer remembers what the questionnaire was

21:03

about, but she can still recall what it

21:05

felt like to make such requests

21:07

of total strangers. Yeah, I mean I

21:09

still have flashbacks of going down to Penn

21:11

Station because it was so distressing. I

21:14

would walk in, there'd be people kind of walking all

21:16

over the place, and then there'd be people just sitting

21:18

down waiting for their trains. So I'd

21:20

usually go up to the person who was sitting

21:22

there waiting for their train, you know, doing whatever

21:24

they do to kind of occupy their time, and

21:27

I would say, excuse me, will you

21:29

please fill out the survey? It felt

21:31

incredibly awkward, stepping into

21:34

someone's space, disturbing them, asking

21:36

them to stop doing what they were doing and

21:39

to do something she wanted them to do. As

21:43

Vanessa asked for help and waited for an answer,

21:45

her palms began to sweat. Her

21:48

heart started beating faster. It was a really

21:50

sort of palpable fear that they were going to

21:52

reject me or worse, right, say

21:54

something mean. I don't even know what, but

21:56

I expected them to say something terrible. Coming

22:00

back on the moment now, it reminds

22:02

her of another Stanley Milgram study, one

22:05

that's less famous than the obedience experiment.

22:07

He had his research

22:10

assistants go onto New York City subways and

22:12

ask people for their seat. Many

22:14

of his students couldn't complete the task. His

22:17

students started coming back to him saying, I can't do

22:19

this, this is just so upsetting, this is the most,

22:21

you know, distressing thing you've ever

22:24

asked me to do. And he was like,

22:26

you guys are being babies, I don't understand

22:28

why this is so upsetting. And

22:30

so, to prove his students wrong, the

22:32

famous researcher set out for the subway himself.

22:36

He would do what his students couldn't,

22:38

walk up to strangers and

22:40

ask them for their seats. He found

22:43

the experience so much more distressing than

22:45

he expected it to be, and all

22:47

of a sudden he understood why they had been complaining so much.

22:50

Why is it so hard to make such requests? Well,

22:53

one obvious explanation is that we know that

22:56

people will reject us and

22:58

that rejection is painful. Vanessa

23:01

remembers being hugely relieved when she was

23:03

done giving out questionnaires at Penn Station

23:06

and could head back to her lab at Columbia

23:08

University. Once there,

23:10

she and her professor, Frank Flynn, analyzed

23:13

the responses to the questionnaire. They

23:16

noticed something intriguing. Frank was like, I

23:18

can't believe how many people are actually saying yes to you.

23:21

Total strangers, disrupted from

23:23

reading their newspaper or eating a sandwich or

23:25

watching the crowds of people in the busy

23:27

station, they were like,

23:30

sure, I'll respond to

23:32

your questionnaire. We were really surprised by

23:34

how many people were agreeing in New York

23:36

Penn Station to do this survey. What

23:39

began as a simple observation turned

23:41

into something much more important, an

23:44

insight about our minds. Here's

23:47

the chain of thought that led to the discovery. The

23:50

reason Frank and Vanessa were surprised that so

23:52

many people said yes is

23:54

because the expected people to

23:57

say no. If

24:01

lots of people said yes, that meant that

24:03

Vanessa's fears about rejection were

24:06

misplaced. Her perception of

24:08

the influence she actually had on other

24:10

people was wrong. Like

24:13

most of us, Vanessa had long felt that others

24:15

had a big effect on her. As

24:18

she gazed at the data, she realized that

24:21

she had a big effect on other people.

24:24

If she was blind to this power, what

24:27

consequences could it have on her behavior?

24:32

As researchers, the first thing that Vanessa and

24:35

Frank decided to do was test if their

24:37

personal experience was

24:39

generalizable. We decided to bring participants

24:42

into the lab and have them do basically what

24:44

I had done on those number of days. So

24:47

we brought them into the lab, we said, hey, we're

24:49

going to have you go out and ask people to,

24:51

as our first step, fill out a survey, just like

24:53

I had done. How many people do you

24:55

think are going to say yes to you? We made them estimate

24:58

how many people they thought would agree, go out

25:00

and actually ask people. What

25:02

we found was that they really underestimated the

25:04

number of people who would agree to that

25:07

request. So it wasn't just

25:09

Vanessa and Frank. People

25:11

in general seemed to have a poor assessment

25:13

of their power over others. People

25:16

thought that others would find it easy to turn

25:18

down their requests. Vanessa

25:20

connected the seeming blind spot in our

25:23

thinking to Stanley Milgram's famous obedience study.

25:26

She realized this might be why everyone always

25:28

saw the experiment from the perspective

25:30

of the volunteers asked to administer

25:32

shocks, the people being

25:34

influenced. No one saw

25:37

the experiment from the point of view of the

25:39

experimenter, the person exercising

25:41

influence. We don't

25:43

ask, was it hard for him to issue

25:46

those crazy instructions because we

25:48

don't identify with people exercising such

25:50

influence. We think that kind

25:52

of person must be very different from us because

25:55

we don't feel we have such

25:57

power. people

26:00

going along with this crazy request he was making

26:02

of them. So it's interesting when

26:04

people think about the Stanley Milgram study,

26:06

and I think this is true for

26:08

myself as well, I always imagine myself

26:10

being in the role of the volunteer

26:12

in the experiment, hearing the instructions from

26:14

the experimenter saying, you must shock this

26:17

other person. I never put myself in

26:19

the shoes of the experimenter. Exactly.

26:21

So that was something that we

26:24

started to wonder about. So we so often

26:26

sort of simulate, if I was in that

26:28

Milgram shock experiment, what would I do if

26:31

I was the study participant? Would I actually

26:33

stand up and go against these directives and

26:35

say no? But we kind of

26:37

flipped that idea on its head. Vanessa

26:40

went back to her experience at Penn Station. It

26:43

felt difficult because she had seen the

26:45

interaction only from the point of view

26:48

of her own insecurities. She

26:50

hadn't seen the encounters through the point of view

26:52

of the people she was asking for help. From

26:55

their perspective, an anxious young woman was

26:58

asking for something trivial. They

27:00

had to weigh whether to put aside what they were

27:02

doing and help her for a few minutes.

27:04

If they said no, it could

27:06

make them look like jerks. It's this

27:08

really interesting phenomenon where you have these

27:11

two people interacting with one another, and

27:13

they're both so focused on their own

27:15

personal anxieties and insecurities and concerns with

27:17

embarrassment that they don't realize that the

27:20

other person is feeling that way too.

27:22

So it's this really interesting situation where

27:24

being so inwardly focused on your own

27:27

anxieties makes it so difficult for you

27:29

to recognize what the situation really is

27:31

for itself. People in

27:33

these encounters experience what psychologists call

27:36

an egocentric bias. They are so

27:38

consumed with their own perceptions that

27:41

they fail to see what the interaction feels

27:43

like for the other person. It's

27:47

absolutely true that many of us are

27:49

influenced by situations that many of us

27:51

will do things because the situation prompts

27:53

it. But there is

27:55

another problem too, and it might be a deeper

27:57

problem. The people who put

27:59

us in those situations, it's not

28:01

like they are all powerful gods. They

28:04

are humans just like us, and

28:06

they may not realize the extent of the power

28:08

they have over us. In

28:10

fact, they may be thinking, I'm

28:12

sure this portion is going to turn down my request. They

28:16

might assume, falsely, that it's

28:18

easy to refuse instructions. Vanessa

28:22

realized that this bias could have

28:24

all sorts of important consequences. So

28:27

what we started looking at about over a

28:29

decade now, ago, we started

28:31

to look at whether we recognize when

28:33

we're the ones who are influencing someone

28:35

else, when we recognize that someone else,

28:38

for example, can't say no to something

28:40

that we've asked them. Vanessa

28:48

is now a psychologist at Cornell

28:50

University. In a series

28:52

of experiments, she has demonstrated how people

28:54

are often oblivious to the power that

28:56

they have over others. In

28:58

one study, she asked volunteers, mostly college

29:01

students, to make a simple request of

29:03

others. We brought people into the lab,

29:05

and we told them, you're going to go out

29:07

into campus and ask people to borrow their phones.

29:10

She walked them through how to approach someone and

29:13

gave them instructions for what to do once

29:15

people agreed to let them use their phones.

29:17

They would call us back at the lab and say,

29:19

I have this person's phone, this is where I'm located.

29:22

We'd mark it down, and then they'd go on and ask

29:24

somebody else. Before the volunteers went

29:26

out to begin the study, Vanessa asked them

29:29

a question. How many people would

29:31

they have to ask to get

29:33

three people to say yes? At

29:35

this time, participants are kind of freaked out by

29:37

this whole thought. They are convinced everyone's going to

29:39

say no, they're not going to be able to

29:41

do the task. Before they actually go out into

29:44

campus to do the task, they often would ask

29:46

us, well, what if no one agrees? Do I

29:48

come back? What do I do? They have all

29:50

these concerns about not being able

29:52

to complete the task. What

29:54

Vanessa found was similar to her own experience

29:56

at Penn Station. Many

29:58

more people said yes. then the

30:00

volunteer is expected. They thought they had to ask a little

30:03

over 10. They actually had to

30:05

ask more like six. In

30:07

fact, every other person was agreeing

30:09

to this request. Maybe

30:11

you think the students had a high success

30:13

rate because they were requesting something trivial. But

30:17

Vanessa has also conducted a version of

30:19

the study where volunteers had to ask

30:21

for something more consequential, many.

30:24

For that study, she enlisted the help

30:26

of the Leukemia and Lymphoma Societies Team

30:29

in Training program. What people do

30:31

when they participate in a fundraising activity for

30:33

team in training is they ask

30:35

people for donations so that they can participate

30:38

in some sort of race, like a triathlon

30:40

or a marathon. They get

30:42

some training and some travel money to be able to

30:44

do that, and the rest of the money actually goes

30:46

to the organization. Vanessa asked participants

30:48

how many people they would have to

30:51

solicit to meet their fundraising goals, which

30:53

were typically thousands of dollars. They

30:56

estimated they would need to ask about 200 people

30:58

to meet the goal. What we found is

31:00

that they actually only had to ask about half that,

31:02

so they only had to ask about 100 people in

31:05

order to reach their fundraising goals. Justice

31:07

in Vanessa's phone study, her

31:09

participants doubled the number of people they thought

31:11

they had to ask to reach their goal.

31:14

Their egocentric bias caused them to focus

31:16

so much on their own anxieties that

31:19

they ignored the influence they actually had

31:21

over other people. You're thinking about what

31:23

you're asking. I'm asking this person for money. Will this

31:25

person give me money? What you're

31:27

not doing is thinking about what if

31:30

you were sitting there, potentially

31:32

in your cubicle, and

31:35

a coworker came up to you and said, hey, I'm

31:37

participating in a race. Would you be willing to sponsor

31:39

me? If you were sitting there,

31:41

it'd be really hard to say no to

31:43

your coworker. It'd be really hard to let

31:45

them down. It'd be really awkward. What would

31:47

you even say? And so people are kind

31:49

of put on the spot and they find

31:52

it really difficult to say no, so they go

31:54

ahead and agree. Thank you. At

32:00

the University of Chicago, economist John

32:02

List has also studied the relationship

32:04

between social pressure and

32:06

charitable giving. John ran

32:09

a study where experimenters knocked on the doors

32:11

of some 8,000 houses in

32:13

the Chicago area. They

32:15

were trying to raise money for a children's

32:17

hospital. John asked me

32:19

to imagine the scenario from the point of view

32:22

of the person receiving the request. Let's

32:24

say it's a Sunday afternoon. I've

32:27

just made myself something to eat. I'm

32:29

relaxed. You're sitting on the couch watching a

32:31

football game and you hear somebody

32:33

knocking on the door and

32:36

you think, okay, should I get up or should

32:38

I stay watching the football games? Of course, a

32:40

lot of people get up and answer the door.

32:43

But once they see that there's a solicitor

32:45

at the door, they say,

32:48

oh my God, I wish I would have

32:50

stayed on the couch watching the football game.

32:52

Too late. If they tell the

32:54

solicitor no, then they

32:56

have this very negative or disutility

32:58

from letting someone down. So

33:01

they're weighing that off versus just

33:04

giving them $20 and having them go on

33:06

their way. John

33:08

added a very interesting twist to the study. Some

33:11

households were told ahead of time that a

33:13

fundraising volunteer would come and knock on the

33:15

door. Others

33:17

were not told ahead of time. They

33:19

just received an unexpected knock. What

33:22

we find is that when we warn them, of

33:24

course, many people just stay on the couch or

33:26

they leave the house. They never answer the door.

33:30

The people who do answer the door,

33:32

they do tend to give money. And

33:35

much of that is because of altruistic

33:37

reasons. But the

33:39

people who we do not warn, they end

33:42

up answering the door more often and they

33:44

give more. Put

33:46

another way, people understand how they're going

33:48

to feel when they're put on the spot.

33:51

They often will go to great lengths to

33:54

avoid getting in such situations. What

33:57

this also means is that some significant

33:59

portion of the money that charities

34:01

raise might not come from

34:03

altruism. In the case of

34:05

the Children's Hospital fundraiser, for example, What

34:08

you find is that roughly three-quarters

34:10

of the dollars given are due

34:14

to social pressure, and a

34:16

quarter of the dollars given is

34:18

actually due to altruism. So a

34:20

very small component of what we

34:22

observe in our door-to-door fundraising drive

34:24

is actually driven by altruism. John's

34:28

research reminds Vanessa of a classic study

34:31

where researchers set up two booths on

34:33

a college campus. One

34:35

booth was clearly asking people for something,

34:37

while the other did not ask for

34:39

anything. What the researchers

34:41

found was similar to John's donation study.

34:44

They measured how far away people walked from the

34:46

booth as they walked by this path, and if

34:48

people knew that they were going to be asked

34:50

for something, their distance from the booth was much

34:53

further than if they didn't think they were going

34:55

to be asked for something. We just kind of

34:57

avoid any chance of having to say no to

34:59

somebody. We've

35:04

seen how egocentric bias can cause us

35:06

to act in helpful ways to others. We

35:09

lend phones to people who need them, or donate

35:11

money to charity. Unfortunately,

35:13

though, there's another side to the

35:15

story. They grabbed that

35:17

headset and they threw it across

35:19

the room. When

35:22

we come back, the sinister side of

35:24

our inability to recognize our power over

35:27

other people. You're listening

35:29

to Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedanta.

35:55

When we interact with others, we are

35:57

often intensely focused on how we feel.

36:00

Our anxieties, our embarrassments,

36:03

our fears. As a

36:05

result, we're often blind to the effect

36:07

we have on others, their

36:10

anxieties, their embarrassments, and

36:12

their fears. Psychologist

36:15

Vanessa Bons has studied how such

36:17

egocentric bias can keep people from

36:20

asking for help. But

36:22

that's not the whole story. There's kind of

36:24

the happy story, which is that people will help

36:27

us more than we think, and

36:29

then there's kind of the darker story, that people

36:31

will do a lot of other things for

36:34

us more so than we think. We've

36:36

run some studies where we started out

36:38

asking people if they could get someone

36:40

to lie for them. Our

36:43

original studies involved just filling something out. We

36:45

said, what if we just have

36:47

them ask if they'll sign their name to

36:49

something saying that you gave them a pitch

36:52

that you didn't actually give them, just kind

36:54

of a white lie. Once

36:56

again, we had people guess how many

36:58

people they would have to make this

37:00

request of before a certain number

37:03

said yes. They went out onto campus. They asked people,

37:05

you know, I'm supposed to be doing this pitch. I

37:07

really don't feel like doing it. Will you just sign

37:09

this thing that I gave you the pitch? And

37:12

again, most people wound up signing it, even though

37:14

our participants thought that most people would say no.

37:18

As Vanessa says, the volunteers were

37:20

asking people to tell a trivial lie. And

37:23

perhaps you could say, what's the big deal

37:25

in signing a note that says someone gave you

37:27

a pitch that they didn't? There

37:29

are no real moral consequences. So

37:32

Vanessa raised the stakes. So

37:35

what we did is we created these fake library

37:37

books. We took a bunch of books off my

37:39

bookshelf and just, you know, put some library

37:42

codes on them. And

37:44

we gave them to participants and we said, we're

37:46

going to have you go into the libraries on

37:48

campus and ask people to vandalize

37:50

these library books. And so

37:52

they were to tell people, I'm playing a prank on

37:55

my friend, but they know my handwriting. Will you please

37:57

just write Pickle in this library book and pen? and

38:00

they left it at that and looked at

38:02

whether or not people agreed. And

38:04

what we found is that the people they approached, so

38:06

they kept track of sort of the things that people

38:08

said when they made this request of them, and

38:11

people would say things like, this is wrong,

38:14

you shouldn't be doing this, we could

38:16

get in trouble. They were clearly uncomfortable

38:18

with the prospect of vandalizing this purported

38:20

library book, but

38:22

they still did it. And

38:27

again, that finding went completely against

38:29

the intuitions of the volunteers doing

38:31

the asking. People

38:33

significantly underestimated how much influence

38:35

they possessed to get others

38:38

to do something unethical. So

38:40

our participants, before they went out and started asking people,

38:42

they thought about 28% of people would agree to do

38:44

this, right? So they thought the

38:46

vast majority of people would say no. But

38:49

when they actually went out and made this request of people,

38:51

64% of the majority of

38:53

the people they asked actually agreed to

38:55

vandalize this library book. I

38:58

mean, that's actually pretty astonishing that 64% of people would

39:00

say yes. I mean, I would not have predicted it

39:02

would be as high a number as that. Yeah,

39:05

I mean, this was a task we designed and

39:07

we were like, this is never going to work,

39:09

right? There's no way people are actually going to

39:11

agree to do this. And we ourselves were completely

39:13

surprised that people did agree. As much

39:16

as it was uncomfortable for them to do

39:18

this unethical thing and vandalize a library book,

39:20

it was way more uncomfortable for them to

39:22

say no to the person who was asking.

39:28

The scenarios in which egocentric bias could

39:30

play a role in our behavior seem

39:32

endless. We share the

39:34

answers to our homework with a friend who asked

39:36

to see our work. We

39:38

don't push back when a colleague suggests bending the

39:41

rules on a timesheet. We

39:43

agree to keep a friend's infidelity a secret,

39:46

even when it makes us uncomfortable. Listening

39:49

to Vanessa made me realize why there is

39:52

a vast gulf between what we predict we

39:54

might do and what we actually

39:56

do when we are confronted with

39:58

problematic behavior. look as

40:00

outsiders at the situation and we say, why would you

40:02

tolerate this? Why wouldn't you just stand up and say,

40:04

I'm not going to do this. I'm not going to

40:07

hurt another person. I need to be able to do

40:09

my job and you're affecting my ability to do my

40:11

job. But in fact, the

40:13

social pressure, the

40:15

concern about offending another person, the

40:18

social anxiety in that situation is

40:20

so palpable to that individual that

40:22

it feels almost impossible for them

40:24

to stand up and say

40:26

something about it and reject the sort of

40:29

behavior that they're encountering. Hidden

40:32

Brain listener Anna Abou-Rouze called in

40:34

with a story that illustrates how

40:36

egocentric bias can affect workplace behavior.

40:39

She was training to be an air

40:42

traffic controller and saw examples of bullying

40:44

and harassing behavior all around her. She

40:47

says the trainers had a

40:49

clear message for trainees. Don't

40:51

be soft or you

40:54

know, you got to have thick skin

40:56

to survive in air traffic. That was a

40:59

common one for sure. You have to have

41:01

thick skin to survive in air traffic. I've

41:03

heard that over a hundred times. Anna

41:05

recalled one painful incident. There

41:08

was a trainee that was trying to

41:10

clear an aircraft for landing and

41:13

the trainer in that moment

41:16

grabbed the headset of the

41:18

trainee and this headset is

41:20

plugged in to the radar

41:23

and they grabbed that headset and

41:26

they threw it across the room,

41:28

which would fly off of the

41:30

head of the trainee. They

41:33

would actually tell them, hey, hurry

41:35

up and go grab it so that you

41:37

can plug back in and clear this aircraft

41:39

for landing. Anna

41:42

says seeing such incidents made her fearful.

41:45

She didn't feel she could complain since

41:47

such behaviors appear to be the norm.

41:50

Who could she complain to? The people

41:52

who were themselves acting badly? They

41:55

would just say things like, what the f*** are you

41:57

doing? If you do that, again,

42:00

I swear to God. One time

42:02

when she was directing aircraft, this was in real

42:04

life not a simulation, she found

42:06

herself sitting next to one of the

42:09

trainers who she says had acted abusively

42:11

toward trainees. By

42:13

this point, Anna was no longer a trainee. She

42:16

was directing two aircraft. One

42:19

was a thousand feet above the

42:21

other. I got the names of

42:23

the aircraft mixed up. And

42:27

I, because I was so nervous, I think, and

42:30

I descended the wrong aircraft.

42:33

I descended the one on top instead of the

42:35

one on the bottom because

42:38

I got those call signs messed up. She

42:41

told the aircraft that was at a

42:43

higher altitude to descend directly

42:46

into the path of the

42:48

lower altitude aircraft. Luckily, the pilot could

42:50

see the aircraft, so the pilot just

42:53

said, no, we're not going to descend. And

42:56

I immediately knew what I had

42:58

just done. And I thought

43:00

today was a clear

43:02

day. It was clear skies. There was no

43:04

clouds in the way. There wasn't any storm clouds in the way.

43:07

But had there been storm clouds or

43:11

had there been some other kind of visual obstruction,

43:14

this plane would have descended and they

43:17

would have hit that aircraft. And

43:19

I would have been responsible for hundreds of deaths. And

43:23

it wasn't because I didn't know how to control the

43:25

traffic. I did. And I had done this

43:27

a million times. It

43:30

was because of the social stress that

43:32

I was in at that time

43:34

that didn't make me think clearly. I

43:41

told Vanessa Barnes what Anna described, how the

43:44

mere presence of the trainer had disrupted her

43:46

to the point where she made a mistake

43:48

that could have been catastrophic. Vanessa

43:51

said, look, it's certainly the

43:53

case that there are lots of unethical people

43:55

who know they are unethical and

43:58

lots of bullies who know they are bullies. Maybe

44:01

that was the case here, but

44:03

there is a deeper problem in the workplace that we

44:05

often forget. The bullies

44:07

and harassers who don't know

44:10

that they are bullies and harassers. Often

44:13

when we're the person causing someone

44:15

else distress, we can't see that

44:17

distress. It's invisible to us, and

44:19

it's not to let anybody off

44:21

the hook because clearly it's the

44:24

people creating this toxic culture's responsibility

44:26

to kind of fix it and

44:28

to not cause these things to

44:30

happen. But there's also

44:33

this cognitive bias there where we

44:35

may not realize the extent to

44:37

which we're interfering with somebody else's

44:39

performance. These same dynamics play

44:41

out in another common occurrence in the

44:43

workplace, unwanted romantic attention.

44:46

We ran a couple of studies

44:48

where we asked people about their

44:50

experiences being asked out at work

44:54

or asking someone out at work. We

44:56

asked people to imagine situations where they weren't interested

44:58

in the other person or the other person wasn't

45:01

interested in them. What

45:03

we found is that people who asked somebody

45:05

out at work and

45:07

were rejected thought that it was pretty easy for

45:09

that person to reject them. They didn't

45:12

think that that person experienced a whole lot of

45:14

distress. They didn't

45:16

think that they changed their behavior very much after

45:18

being asked out. But when

45:20

people recall situations where they were asked out

45:22

by someone at work who they weren't interested

45:24

in, they described feeling obligated

45:26

to say yes, feeling much more uncomfortable

45:28

saying no to the person, and they

45:30

reported doing all sorts of things to

45:32

try to avoid that person that the

45:34

other person didn't realize that they were

45:36

doing. In fact,

45:38

this little request, we tell people to just

45:41

go for it and ask this person out,

45:44

it actually puts a lot more pressure on the other

45:46

person than we tend to realize when we're the

45:48

ones doing the asking. In some ways,

45:50

we underestimate the pressure that we exert on other people.

45:53

In some ways, that's the moral of the whole story

45:55

here, isn't it? Absolutely. Yeah.

45:58

We underestimate the... influence

46:00

that we have over other people, and

46:02

we underestimate the extent to which asking

46:04

them for something really puts them in

46:06

an awkward position, because now they

46:09

have to say no, and that's just a really hard thing

46:11

for people to do. Like

46:14

many psychological biases, the tendency we

46:16

have to downplay the influence we

46:18

have on others can have far-reaching

46:20

consequences. It can keep

46:22

us from asking for help that would be forthcoming.

46:26

It can keep us from reaching out and making

46:28

friends with strangers. And

46:30

it can also lead us to give in

46:32

to unethical demands or make improper demands of

46:34

other people. I

46:36

asked Vanessa how her research had

46:38

prompted her to do things differently

46:40

in her own life. It has made a

46:42

huge difference. And the little

46:45

things, so for example, when I was

46:47

pregnant, if I needed a

46:49

seat on the subway or on a

46:51

train, I would kind of stand there

46:53

and look around and try to look

46:55

my most pathetic so that someone would

46:57

give me their seat, thinking that someone

46:59

would step up and do it because

47:01

they were nice, right? But

47:03

in fact, everyone's all involved in their

47:05

own stuff. They're not necessarily looking around

47:07

and paying attention. And maybe they'd

47:09

be perfectly happy to give up their seat, but they're

47:12

not going to think of it unless you actually ask.

47:15

And so I tried to take that into account. So when

47:17

I was pregnant, I would go up to people and be

47:19

like, hey, can I sit down? I'd really

47:21

love to sit. And then of course, people

47:23

are incredibly happy to just pop up and say yes. And

47:26

what's interesting, of course, is when that happens, you're actually

47:28

giving people an opportunity to do something nice. It's not

47:30

just that you're imposing on them. Presumably, some of them

47:32

are actually happy to say, you know, I was just

47:35

writing to work and now I actually got to do

47:37

this nice thing for this other person. I feel this

47:39

little warm glow. Yeah, absolutely.

47:41

So a lot of people wonder about the

47:43

takeaway. So if people agree to

47:46

help us out of obligation because they feel like

47:48

they can't say no, then do

47:50

you really want to ask them for things? But

47:52

people are really good at justifying their behaviors and

47:54

ways that make them feel good about themselves. So

47:57

they may agree to help because they feel like they

47:59

can't. say no, but pretty quickly after that they're

48:02

going to be convincing themselves that they helped because

48:04

they're a really wonderful person. And so everyone's going

48:06

to walk away feeling good about the interaction. You

48:08

got the help that you needed and the other

48:10

person gets to feel like a good person. Psychologists

48:18

once conducted a lighthearted version of

48:20

Stanley Milgram's obedience study. In

48:22

the 1970s, they had research assistants stand

48:24

on the streets of New York City.

48:27

Their jobs? To look skyward.

48:31

At nothing. The question

48:33

the researchers wanted to know was whether

48:35

innocent passers-by would also stop to look

48:37

up to see what was going on. They

48:41

found that when more people were in on the

48:43

gag, more pedestrians stopped and

48:45

looked up. I've

48:48

seen video of that study many times and

48:50

always found this scene funny. One

48:52

to fifteen people just staring off into the

48:55

sky. Recently, I

48:57

rewatched it and this time

48:59

I did what Vanessa had done. I

49:02

flipped the script. Instead

49:04

of seeing the experiment from the point

49:06

of view of the passers-by and asking

49:08

myself whether I would be similarly influenced,

49:12

I looked at the experiment from the point

49:14

of view of the research assistants. Did

49:17

they expect so many people to join them in

49:19

looking at nothing? We've

49:25

seen throughout this episode how all of

49:27

us as individuals have great power to

49:29

shape how others behave. If

49:32

each of us has this hidden

49:34

power, then collectively as groups, as

49:36

communities, as tribes, we are

49:38

going to have even more influence. How

49:42

we choose to use that influence? That's

49:44

up to each of us. Hidden

50:00

Brain is produced by Hidden Brain Media. Our

50:03

audio production team includes Annie

50:05

Murphy-Paugh, Kristin Wong, Laura Quirell,

50:07

Ryan Katz, Autumn Barnes, Andrew

50:10

Chadwick, and Nick Woodbury. Tara

50:12

Boyle is our executive producer. I'm

50:15

Hidden Brain's executive editor. We

50:18

end today with a story from our sister show,

50:20

My Unsunk Hero. It's brought

50:23

to you by T-Mobile for Business. Our

50:26

story comes from Bethany Renfri. I

50:28

was 20 years old. My baby girls

50:31

and I were living in low-income apartments.

50:34

Most of my neighbors were single mothers

50:36

like myself. And

50:38

I remember how overwhelmed I felt that morning.

50:40

It was a cold day in the apartment.

50:43

I dragged myself to the sink, and

50:46

it was stacked with dishes, with

50:48

pots and pans that had been soaking because

50:50

I'd burnt them all. I didn't know how

50:52

to cook back then, and I would always

50:55

burn our pans. And

50:57

I looked at my twin girls.

50:59

They were 18 months old. They sat in

51:01

their high chairs. The baby, the

51:04

newborn, was in her swing. I

51:06

looked back at the sink, and

51:08

I just couldn't bring myself to do those

51:10

dishes, and I couldn't look at

51:12

them any longer. It was

51:15

a reminder of how overwhelmed I

51:17

felt in my own life. So

51:20

I grabbed a white garbage bag, and I

51:22

stacked the dishes in there one by one.

51:25

I walked out in the rain, and I placed

51:27

it on the edge of the apartment dumpster because

51:30

the dumpster was full. And

51:32

I came back in, and the girls and I left for

51:34

the day. When I got back

51:36

that evening, it was dark, and my

51:38

porch was dark because I didn't even have

51:40

the energy to change the porch

51:42

light. But as we

51:45

were coming in, I kind of kicked something. It was

51:47

a box. And so I

51:49

brought it into the apartment and put it on the

51:51

table, and it was

51:53

my pots and pans, and

51:56

they were shining and sparkling, and

51:58

the girls' blues clues. plates and their

52:01

sippy cups and

52:03

a little handwritten note popped out on a

52:05

yellow piece of paper and it

52:08

said, I've been there

52:10

before you will make it

52:12

I promise you. I

52:17

don't know which of the single mothers

52:19

went out there that day and saw

52:22

that garbage bag and

52:24

understood what was happening but

52:26

if I saw her today I would

52:29

thank her for showing me that

52:31

we are not alone and we

52:34

are not bad mothers even in

52:36

our hardest moments. We

52:38

are surrounded by kindness and

52:40

understanding and I'm so

52:43

grateful to have learned that

52:45

lesson so early on in motherhood. Bethany

52:56

Renfri lives in Sutter Creek, California.

52:59

When she was 27 she went back

53:01

to school and earned undergraduate and master's

53:04

degrees. She is now

53:06

a legislative director in the California State

53:08

Senate. Today's

53:10

My Unsung Hero story was brought to

53:12

you by T-Mobile for Business. If

53:17

you enjoy Hidden Brain please take a moment and

53:19

share your favorite episode with two or three people

53:21

in your life. Word of

53:23

mouth recommendations really make a huge difference

53:26

in introducing new listeners to our work.

53:29

We truly appreciate your support. I'm

53:35

Shankar V. Danthem. See you soon. you

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features