Podchaser Logo
Home
3,000 Types Of Brain Cells Categorized In Massive Brain Cell Atlas

3,000 Types Of Brain Cells Categorized In Massive Brain Cell Atlas

Released Thursday, 18th January 2024
 1 person rated this episode
3,000 Types Of Brain Cells Categorized In Massive Brain Cell Atlas

3,000 Types Of Brain Cells Categorized In Massive Brain Cell Atlas

3,000 Types Of Brain Cells Categorized In Massive Brain Cell Atlas

3,000 Types Of Brain Cells Categorized In Massive Brain Cell Atlas

Thursday, 18th January 2024
 1 person rated this episode
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

At the center of our culture war

0:02

lies a single word, woe. I'm Kai

0:04

Wright. Join me on Notes from America,

0:06

live from Harlem's Apollo Theater, to explore

0:08

the word's origins as we celebrate Dr.

0:10

Martin Luther King Jr. Listen

0:12

wherever you get your podcasts. We're

0:19

one step closer to understanding the

0:21

complexity of the human brain. Instead

0:24

of characterizing cells on the basis

0:26

of their shape or who they

0:28

connect with or their firing properties,

0:30

instead one can characterize them on the basis of the

0:33

sets of genes that they use. I'm

0:35

sci-fi producer Shoshana Bucksbaum. It's Thursday,

0:37

January 18th, and we've

0:39

got our brain cells firing on all

0:42

cylinders because today is Science Friday. Late

0:46

last year, scientists released an impressively

0:48

detailed map of human brain cells.

0:51

Considering the human brain has a little over 170 billion

0:53

cells, it's a monumental task made possible

0:58

by an international group of

1:00

scientists. They've identified 3,000 different

1:03

types of cells. Ira Flatow and sci-fi

1:06

producer Kathleen Davis talk with one of

1:08

the scientists who worked on the project

1:10

and take listener calls. Dr.

1:13

Ed Lean, senior investigator at the

1:16

Allen Institute for Brain Science based

1:18

in Seattle. Welcome to Science Friday.

1:21

Good afternoon. Thank you for having me. Dr.

1:24

Lean, just how big an advance is

1:26

this cell atlas for the field of

1:28

neuroscience? Are we talking about a human

1:30

genome project level paradigm shift here? Yes,

1:34

I believe that's a really good analogy

1:36

for the advance made through this work

1:38

as the first installment really. One

1:42

of the problems with neuroscience is

1:44

the extreme complexity of the brain.

1:47

It's just very difficult to study for

1:50

the human brain the scale and the

1:52

inaccessibility of studying the brain are

1:55

serious barriers. We really

1:57

needed a technology breakthrough to be able

1:59

to Handle this complexity. And

2:02

a breakthrough am very it up appropriate

2:04

for the genome. References actually come from

2:06

the feel genomics. Where. Instead

2:09

of characterizing cells on the basis

2:11

of their shape, Or. Who

2:13

they connect with or they're firing properties.

2:16

In. Said one can characterize them on the basis of

2:18

the sets of genes that they use. For.

2:20

Every selling in the body has

2:22

all genes in their dna. By.

2:25

Any given Cel only uses a

2:27

subset of those teams, and that

2:29

molecular fingerprint of itself is a

2:31

really strong way to be able

2:33

to classify cells. As as

2:36

the field of To Know Max has advanced,

2:38

what's happened is that sequencing has become cheaper

2:40

and cheaper. and cheaper. And has

2:42

been miniaturised to be able to in

2:44

to analyze individual cells. And

2:46

now it's possible to analyze all the

2:49

genes being actively use by millions of

2:51

cells. And this now let's

2:53

you. Take. A much broader scope

2:55

of trying to categorize the complexity of

2:57

the brain. And so this

2:59

first installment was really trying to. Get.

3:02

A first draft by sampling about a hundred

3:04

regions of the brain and and then ask

3:06

how many types of cells are there and

3:08

you know I think. For. For

3:11

really over a century we understood that

3:13

the complexity is high but not quite

3:15

as high in other get see that

3:17

thousands of types of cells. So three

3:19

thousand. As you mentioned here, I'm.

3:22

Even since the time that publication, a comprehensive

3:24

analysis of the smaller mouse brain has come

3:27

a. Discovery or Thousand Celta had sexually.

3:29

We're going to get into that. After the break

3:31

we have to take a break so stay with.

3:34

Seductively. In there are, as you

3:36

were saying before the break, there

3:38

are one hundred and seventy one

3:40

billion cells in the human brain

3:42

Cells you possibly. Go about categorizing all

3:44

of them. Yet,

3:47

so it's true that there is that many

3:49

cells, and about half of those are neurons.

3:52

And but they don't come in that many different

3:54

types of cells. and so that

3:57

the key advance here is to be

3:59

able to category the cells into

4:02

groups of cells that have

4:05

different parts of the brain, that have different properties, and

4:08

make a catalog of these and a

4:10

map of these. And so these

4:12

new technologies, as I was describing, are

4:15

allowing us to create these so-called cell atlases.

4:17

And on the one hand, this is a

4:19

classification of the types where

4:22

we can define how many types of cells there

4:24

are, what their relative proportions are in different parts

4:26

of the brain. We can describe

4:28

their spatial organization now, how

4:31

they're organized in local areas

4:34

and also globally in the three-dimensional structure of

4:36

the brain, and begin to characterize

4:38

their properties and their function. And

4:40

so in many ways, this is really

4:42

like the genome project in the sense

4:44

of the genome characterized all of the

4:46

genes and their locations on the chromosomes.

4:49

Here we're characterizing all of the cells and

4:51

their locations and eventually their function in

4:53

the brain. One of the surprises of

4:55

the Human Genome Project was just about

4:57

how many genes there were, a surprising

5:00

less number than people thought. Are

5:03

you being surprised by how many different types of

5:05

cells you're finding in the brain? Most

5:09

definitely. These sort

5:11

of molecular approaches to classify

5:13

cells are revealing a

5:15

whole different level of complexity, probably an

5:18

order of magnitude more than we had realized

5:20

before. And importantly, one

5:23

or two orders of magnitude, so 10 to 100 times

5:25

more complex than any other organ in

5:28

the body. I was just going

5:30

to ask, I mean, how does this go to

5:32

other parts of our body? I can't imagine that,

5:34

like our biceps, for example, have 3,000

5:36

different types of cells. No,

5:39

nowhere near as many types of cells. So

5:41

the complexity is really very high. But

5:43

importantly, actually, these techniques,

5:46

by using genes as the way to define

5:48

cells, this technology can

5:50

be applied to any organ system. And so

5:52

in addition to the brain, in

5:54

parallel, there are efforts happening using these same

5:56

technologies in every other organ system, any

5:59

of the compiled. for example, in a project

6:01

called the Human Cell Atlas to try to put

6:03

all of this together. So we finally have a

6:05

common language to talk about the basic units of

6:07

life, the cells that make up every organ in

6:10

the body, but the complexity factor is

6:12

much higher in the brain. How

6:15

many new kinds of brain cells have you

6:17

discovered that we didn't know existed? And

6:20

the second part would be how many more do you think

6:22

are out there? Yes,

6:26

so these are somewhat difficult questions

6:28

to answer because

6:30

we have a new way of looking at these cells.

6:33

And so when we describe 3,000 types of

6:35

cells, in those parts of the brain that

6:37

we understand well, we can see that this actually

6:41

maps very well to what was known before,

6:44

but adds another level of resolution on this.

6:46

And so maybe you might have

6:48

thought that there were 50 types of cells in

6:51

the part of the neocortex. Now we see

6:53

there may be 150 types of cells. But

6:56

in other parts of the brain that we don't understand as well,

6:59

a lot of this is brand new knowledge. And

7:01

so we don't really know what this means yet.

7:04

But we have the framework now where

7:06

investigators across the whole community can come

7:08

and begin to add information to this.

7:11

So again, very much like the genome where at

7:13

first the genes were mapped and then function was

7:15

laid on top of that, this is

7:17

what's going to happen now. Now that we've defined

7:19

a blueprint of the cell types, now

7:21

we can start to understand what they are and what they

7:23

do. Great. That's great to hear.

7:26

Allison in Erie, Pennsylvania, welcome to Science

7:28

Friday. I'm inspired by

7:30

the limits of things. I know

7:32

Einstein's all things with thought experiments.

7:34

And lately I've

7:36

been appreciating how we can go

7:39

farther when we acknowledge what we don't. My

7:42

metaphor to start is just if

7:44

we smash open a radio, of

7:46

course the machine parts don't necessarily

7:50

give us all of the answers as to

7:53

the content that's coming through the radio, for

7:55

example. I just

7:57

wondered what this inspires in your

7:59

work. you and you're thinking about neuroscience and

8:02

I know we're talking about the cells but

8:04

what are the implications for you? I've

8:06

heard that this is paradigm shift. I'm

8:09

just curious what this inspires you,

8:11

whatever you found, how it's

8:14

inspiring your creativity in the

8:16

space in neuroscience, what it's

8:18

inspiring you to think about

8:20

or consider and it might

8:22

be a sensitive question because you're a scientist so

8:24

often you're not going to talk about things until

8:26

they're proven but I kind of wanted to challenge

8:28

you to talk about that. What

8:30

are some things that this inspires you

8:33

to think about or

8:35

even consider? Again, I'm not to challenge

8:37

the science of what's proven or not

8:39

proven but I know it's important

8:41

to be creative in the space and

8:43

metaphors help me a lot but yeah

8:45

it's exciting what we don't know and

8:48

just curious what it's inspiring you to

8:50

consider or think about. Good question. Let's get

8:52

the answer to that. What do you say

8:54

to that? Yes, that's

8:56

a very interesting question. First of

8:58

all, let me acknowledge

9:01

that this is really a reductionist approach to

9:03

the brain. In your

9:05

analogy of a radio, we take

9:08

it, we deconstruct its parts and we try to

9:10

understand its parts and very

9:12

much like that analogy, this

9:15

is just the beginning. Now

9:18

we know that the neurons

9:20

of the brain form circuits,

9:22

complicated circuits which are by

9:24

definition the connections between the

9:26

different kinds of cells. What

9:29

this really sets the stage

9:31

for is beginning to take

9:33

the next step. If

9:35

we don't understand the basic components, we can't

9:37

understand how they connect together. But

9:40

now that we understand that part of it,

9:42

we can move to the next stage. Even

9:45

that may not be the end of the day. There

9:48

may be software

9:50

on top of that that

9:52

actually dictates the function of this.

9:54

I think that this

9:57

is really just beginning but it is

9:59

an essential component. component of the process

10:01

here and one that we haven't had

10:03

before. If you don't understand the basic

10:05

building blocks, you can't understand how

10:07

it all fits together to work. And

10:11

I think that what's

10:13

important to consider here is that we've

10:15

had this sort of dearth of understanding

10:17

of the fine detailed structure of the

10:19

brain and that's dramatically hampered

10:22

our understanding of disease. And

10:25

this type of resource can now

10:28

allow us to bring this level of

10:30

resolution to understanding exactly what happens in

10:32

disease. And so to give a concrete

10:35

example and something I'm quite passionate about is

10:38

with this information, we can

10:41

think of cells as things that are

10:43

affected in disease that may in fact

10:45

be targets for treatment of disease. So

10:47

we have another project focused on

10:50

Alzheimer's disease called the Seattle Alzheimer's

10:52

Disease Brain Cell Outlets or CAD

10:55

where the idea is let's now look

10:57

at the brain of individuals that have Alzheimer's

10:59

disease and try to understand what

11:02

kinds of cells are affected in disease.

11:05

And we can map against this reference

11:07

and then ask questions about what the

11:09

real basis of disease is. Whereas

11:11

in the past, when we think

11:14

about pathological proteins, plaques and tangles

11:16

that everyone knows about that just

11:18

haven't worked as a way of

11:20

treating disease. And what we find is

11:22

that we can find all of these types

11:24

of cells and individuals with disease, we

11:27

can then see that certain types of

11:29

cells are differentially affected or vulnerable. And

11:32

so this is a kind of a different way

11:34

of thinking of things. It's thinking of the brain

11:36

as a cellular organ where specific

11:38

components do different things, they're differentially

11:40

affected by disease and

11:42

they become actual targets themselves. And

11:45

something else that comes from these atlases

11:47

is the ability to develop tools to

11:49

target particular cells and potentially deliver genetic

11:52

therapies to them. So

11:54

I view this as it's not a

11:56

stem collecting exercise. This is really foundational

11:59

work. that defines the system so that

12:01

we can understand

12:04

and treat disease. So how

12:06

would that work practically if you are trying

12:08

to treat something like Alzheimer's? I

12:11

mean how would that treatment potentially

12:13

work with this knowledge? Yeah,

12:17

so Alzheimer's is maybe a difficult example. It happens

12:19

to be one that I know a lot about.

12:22

But let me give a more

12:25

easy example perhaps of epilepsy.

12:28

So epilepsy is of course

12:31

an imbalance of excitation and

12:33

inhibition and often affects the

12:35

inhibitory cells. There's not enough

12:37

inhibition and you get

12:39

runaway excitation that's a seizure activity. One

12:43

of the things that's come out of this

12:45

cell atlas work is that we

12:48

can not only understand what genes are

12:50

active in what cells, but

12:52

how they're regulated to be so be

12:55

active only in those cells. So we

12:57

can identify the regulatory regions of the

12:59

genome that are responsible for turning on a

13:02

gene in only a certain kind of cell. That

13:05

regulatory element can actually be put

13:07

into a virus as a means

13:10

of gene delivery that's commonly

13:12

used in gene therapies now, such

13:14

as adenocessiated virus. These regulatory

13:17

domains can be put in to

13:19

turn on expression of a particular

13:21

gene, say a gene replacement of a

13:23

genetic epilepsy. And so you

13:25

can infect cells in people and

13:29

deliver a gene just to the right kinds

13:31

of cells that may be able to

13:33

correct those seizure phenotypes.

13:37

And this is just one example. Many types

13:39

of brain diseases will affect specific kinds of

13:41

cells. And what I'm

13:43

trying to convey is we can now

13:45

harness this information of this descriptive atlas

13:48

to build a tool to target the right cell type and

13:50

hopefully correct the genetic deficiency of some

13:52

sort without having side effects or off-target

13:55

effects that happen by hitting the wrong

13:57

kinds of cells. Really interesting.

14:00

technology. Let's go to the phones to

14:03

Ry in Houston. Hi, Ry. Yeah,

14:06

how are you, Ira? Thanks for taking my call. Yeah,

14:08

you're welcome. Go ahead. Yeah,

14:11

so my question is, has there

14:13

been any study of the connection

14:15

between the gut and the brain,

14:17

and is there any genomic similarities

14:19

that might, you know, create a

14:22

relationship or suggest one? Microbiome, our

14:24

favorite topic here at Science Friday.

14:26

Good question. Yeah,

14:28

that is an excellent question that

14:30

I'm afraid I'm somewhat ill-equipped to

14:33

answer. But let

14:35

me just say that I think that the data

14:38

are now becoming available to allow that

14:40

kind of question to be asked. For

14:43

example, in this human cell outlets that

14:45

I was mentioning earlier, there are efforts

14:47

to profile all the cells in the

14:49

gut and the immune system. And

14:52

so by virtue of

14:54

using these same types of technologies

14:56

to look at genetic similarities among

14:58

different kinds of cells anywhere

15:00

in the body, it will be possible to do

15:02

this and we can begin to start to see

15:04

where these functional links may be. But

15:07

I have to speak in generalities because I'm not aware

15:09

of studies that have done that today. I

15:11

mean, sometimes big projects like this raise more

15:14

questions than they answer. How much

15:16

of this project helps you better

15:19

understand the brain versus just opened

15:21

up more questions? Oh,

15:25

this is a huge advance in

15:27

understanding the brain. You know,

15:30

as I mentioned sort of before, you

15:32

know, this really forms a scaffold. It

15:34

forms a cellular framework. But

15:37

at the moment, these are cells that are

15:39

defined by genes with these kinds of methods.

15:41

And that's a very

15:43

powerful way of understanding a cell because the

15:45

set of genes that are selectively used by

15:48

a cell are the genes that

15:50

are responsible for the properties of those cells. So

15:52

this is much more information than simply saying,

15:55

you know, here's the shape of a cell.

15:57

We're going to categorize based on the shapes.

16:00

You can't do very much with that information. But

16:02

if you have all of the genes being asked,

16:04

now you can use this in a thousand different

16:06

ways. What drugs might

16:08

act on molecules that are

16:10

expressed only in certain kinds of cells, for

16:12

example? Up first achieves the

16:15

rare one-two punches of being

16:17

short and thorough, national and

16:19

international, fact-based and personable. Every

16:22

morning, we take the three biggest stories of the

16:24

day and explain why they matter. And we do

16:27

it all in less than 15 minutes. So

16:29

you can start your day a little more in the know

16:31

than when you went to sleep. Listen now

16:34

to the Up First podcast from NPR. What

16:38

about cells that are not actually in the

16:40

brain? For example, I've heard that your retina

16:43

is really part of your brain. We

16:45

have neurons that go through our spinal cord. We

16:47

talked about the gut a little bit, solar plexus.

16:50

Will those cells be included in

16:52

this catalog? So

16:55

at the moment, this particular effort is focused

16:58

on the central nervous system. Right.

17:00

Minus the retina. So the retina is actually part of

17:02

the central nervous system, but it's not being included. However,

17:05

it does profile or characterize

17:09

any kind of cell that's present in the brain,

17:11

whether or not this is an actual brain cell

17:14

or a circulating cell in the vasculature,

17:16

for example. And so actually,

17:18

one of the things to come out of this is that

17:21

there's a greater diversity of cells that

17:24

aren't neurons that are in the brain as

17:27

well. And so this really does give

17:29

you a very comprehensive view of the

17:31

overall makeup of the organ. And

17:34

I'm sorry, I've neglected to complete my

17:36

answer on the last days about whether we understand the brain.

17:42

With this scaffold, now you can have a very targeted approach

17:44

to start to characterize the properties of all these cells. So

17:47

the gene expression part of it is very informative, but

17:49

the next stage is what do these cells look like? Who

17:53

do they connect to? What are their firing

17:56

properties? What are their functions? And so now we're going to

17:58

talk about the brain cells. Now

18:00

that you are able to pin identities on

18:02

these cells, you can make very targeted inquiries

18:04

to start to annotate this or interpret your

18:06

functional experiments in light of what

18:09

kinds of cells are actually active in

18:11

a behavior. If you could, I'm going to give you

18:13

my blank check, if you had an enormous amount of

18:15

money to do or buy or create some kind of

18:17

tool to study what you want to know, what would

18:19

it be? I

18:24

think that I would actually invest

18:26

in the utility

18:28

of creating these tools to

18:30

target particular kinds of cells

18:33

that can help to understand the brain and treat

18:36

disease. I think there's an

18:38

enormous new field of precision medicine

18:40

that's being opened up by this

18:42

information. This

18:45

has already actually become, it

18:48

has become a huge priority for

18:50

the NIH that

18:52

is investing in another program as

18:54

part of its brain initiative that's

18:56

called the armamentarium or sort of

18:58

an arsenal of tools to be able

19:01

to genetically target and manipulate different kinds

19:03

of cells in the nervous system. I

19:06

think this is going to be enormously

19:08

important for medicine where we

19:10

now get a cellular understanding of

19:13

different diseases and can actually

19:15

target and try to correct those diseases.

19:17

Wow, wow, this is certainly exciting for

19:19

personalized medicine. Thank you, Dr. Aleen, for

19:21

the work that you do. Thank

19:25

you very much. That's it for today.

19:27

A lot of folks helped make the show

19:29

happen, including Jordan Smudgik, Charles

19:31

Bergquist, George Harper, John

19:33

Dankoski, and many more. Tomorrow, a

19:36

roundup of the top science news

19:38

of the week. I'm Sci-fi

19:40

producer Shoshana Bucksbaum. Catch you next

19:42

time.

Rate

Join Podchaser to...

  • Rate podcasts and episodes
  • Follow podcasts and creators
  • Create podcast and episode lists
  • & much more

Episode Tags

Do you host or manage this podcast?
Claim and edit this page to your liking.
,

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features