Podchaser Logo
Home
[RERELEASE] What the Words Say

[RERELEASE] What the Words Say

BonusReleased Thursday, 6th April 2023
Good episode? Give it some love!
[RERELEASE] What the Words Say

[RERELEASE] What the Words Say

[RERELEASE] What the Words Say

[RERELEASE] What the Words Say

BonusThursday, 6th April 2023
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:05

Hi, it's Emily again, host

0:07

of Sold a Story. If you're just finding

0:10

this podcast, please stop, go

0:12

back, and start with episode one.

0:15

If you're looking for more now

0:17

that you've finished Sold a Story, you're in

0:19

the right place. I made four

0:21

documentaries about reading before

0:24

Sold a Story. This is the fourth of

0:26

the four.

0:27

Making these documentaries

0:29

is what led us to make Sold a Story.

0:32

We will have a bonus episode of that

0:34

podcast

0:35

coming soon. In the meantime,

0:37

this is What the Words Say.

0:40

It was first released on August 6, 2020.

0:44

From American Public Media, this is an APM

0:46

Reports documentary. I'm Emily Hanford. Two

0:49

Two years ago, I visited a juvenile detention

0:51

facility in Houston called the Burnett-Bailand

0:54

Rehabilitation Center. It's

0:56

a large brown building surrounded by metal

0:58

fencing. Welcome

0:59

to BBRC. I'm led

1:01

through two large locked doors. We

1:04

are a secured facility. We

1:06

have both pre-adjudicated

1:08

kids here, meaning before they go to court

1:10

and post-adjudicated kids. Once

1:12

they've gone to court, they come here to serve

1:14

out their time and to get treatment. I'm

1:16

getting a tour from Jennifer Hunley, the assistant

1:19

administrator. It's all boys here,

1:21

as young as 10. They walk the halls

1:23

in blue jumpsuits, their hands clasped behind

1:25

their backs. We pass the units where

1:27

they sleep on thin plastic mattresses, and

1:30

the isolation room where they're sent when their behavior

1:32

is out of control. And

1:35

then we get to a windowless cinder block room

1:37

with heavy locked doors on each side. There's

1:39

a table and two green chairs. This

1:42

is where the boys get to visit with their families once

1:44

a week. It's also where some of them

1:46

are learning to read. So you can do one

1:48

word, one sentence at a time and

1:51

then create the story and I can help you.

1:53

A tutor is sitting across the table from one

1:56

of the kids locked up here at BBRC. I'm

1:58

not allowed to ask why he's here.

2:00

or use his real name. I'll call him DeShawn.

2:02

He's 17. All that noise you

2:04

hear in the background is a kid banging on something

2:06

in the hallway. Each lesson

2:08

begins with some instruction, things like how

2:10

two letters can blend together to make one sound.

2:13

Then the student does some writing and some reading.

2:16

Making and keeping friends can be

2:18

hard, can be hard work. We

2:21

can do many things to help keep our

2:24

friendship strong. We can cheer

2:26

our friends

2:26

on. DeShawn says he's learning a lot of things in

2:28

these lessons that he never knew. Okay,

2:30

like, P.H. It's

2:32

a, I never knew that, like, they

2:35

said, like an F. You know, when you put a P.H.

2:37

together, that's like physics, you

2:39

know what I'm saying? Like that.

2:41

Deshaun is getting this tutoring as part

2:43

of a study being conducted by researchers at

2:45

the University of Houston. The researchers

2:47

are investigating the relationship between reading

2:50

problems and involvement in the juvenile justice

2:52

system. These are kids who are reading at

2:54

or below the third grade level. This is Leslie

2:56

Hart, one of the researchers working on the study. There

2:59

are an awful lot of kids who are coming in

3:01

who simply can't read at all.

3:03

A lot of them have learning disabilities that were never

3:06

identified, says Latasha Crenshaw. She

3:08

worked for the juvenile probation department, advocating

3:11

on behalf of kids in the justice system who need

3:13

special education services. She

3:15

told me when she talked to their parents, they would

3:17

say things like this. I knew that my

3:20

son had a problem in first

3:22

grade when I was coming up to the

3:24

school every day, telling you something

3:27

was wrong and no one listened. So, you

3:29

know, and for many parents, we get tears,

3:32

like I was right.

3:33

I knew, and

3:36

my child is finally getting the help. And then we

3:38

get the tears of the, but

3:40

they're in the justice system, when

3:43

all this possibly could have

3:45

been avoided. She says when she'd review

3:47

student records, she'd often see a pattern

3:49

that starts in elementary school. When

3:51

kids are having a hard time learning, they

3:54

act up.

3:54

Henry Gonzalez, who was Assistant

3:57

Executive Director of the Juvenile Probation

3:59

Department when I. visited, says behavior

4:01

problems and reading problems go together

4:03

all the time. I don't know how to read and

4:06

I don't want everyone to know about that, but

4:08

I know how to make you laugh, therefore I'm going to be the class

4:10

clown. I don't know how to do these things,

4:12

but I can fight, therefore I'm going to beat you

4:14

up.

4:15

Not all struggling readers act out, of course.

4:17

Some withdraw, stay quiet, hope no one will

4:19

notice. The research on the links

4:22

between reading problems and social and emotional

4:24

problems is sobering.

4:26

Reading readers are more likely to say

4:28

they are sad, angry, lonely,

4:30

and depressed. They're also less likely

4:32

to graduate from high school and more likely

4:34

to end up in the criminal justice system. After

4:40

Deshawn's reading lesson, I got a chance to interview

4:42

him. So what do

4:44

you remember about reading when you were first learning

4:46

how to read?

4:47

That it was hard. That's

4:50

really it. Tell

4:52

me more about that. What was hard? What

4:54

did it feel like? Like when I was just reading,

4:56

I just didn't know none of the words. Like

4:59

the only reason I knew how to read a little bit is because

5:01

I hear people talk, you know? Like

5:04

I knew I could, I see the words, I can, you know what I'm saying?

5:07

So you like memorized the words? Memorized

5:10

the words.

5:11

This is what a lot of struggling readers have

5:13

told me. They memorize words, store

5:16

them like pictures in their mind. But

5:18

there are tens of thousands of words in the

5:20

English language. You can't memorize

5:22

them all. Research shows you need

5:24

to understand the relationships between letters

5:26

and sounds. That's why Deshawn

5:28

is working on things like understanding that pH

5:31

makes a fff sound. Deshawn

5:33

says he wants to be a better reader.

5:35

Can't be no flunky. I don't want

5:37

to be a bum, you know. Trying

5:40

to take care of myself. I don't want to be out there

5:42

on the streets.

5:43

Deshawn wants to go back to high school when he

5:45

gets out to get his diploma. But the sad

5:47

truth is most kids in the juvenile justice

5:50

system never graduate from high school. from high school. One

5:52

study found that 49% of juveniles

5:55

who'd been in detention were in an adult prison

5:57

by the time they were 25.

6:04

From APM reports, this is what the words

6:06

say. For the past several years,

6:08

I've been reporting on what scientists have figured

6:11

out about how skilled reading works, and

6:13

the fact that a lot of teachers aren't being taught

6:15

the scientific research in their preparation

6:17

programs or on the job. I've

6:20

found that some of what teachers learn is actually

6:22

at odds with the scientific research. Why

6:25

is this happening? In large part,

6:27

it's because reading instruction is political

6:30

and has been for a long time. The

6:32

basic debate comes down to a centuries-old

6:35

chicken or the egg argument about what it takes

6:37

for kids to be able to understand what they read.

6:40

One side said, start with letters and sounds.

6:43

If kids know how to decode words, reading

6:45

comprehension will follow. The

6:47

other side said, no. If you focus too

6:49

much on the letters and sounds, kids won't pay attention

6:52

to the meaning of what they're reading. Focus

6:54

on comprehension. This

6:56

debate misunderstands what cognitive

6:58

scientists have figured out about how reading

7:01

comprehension works. This hour

7:03

I'm going to tell you about what they've learned.

7:04

Their research not only sheds

7:06

light on what children need to learn to become

7:08

good readers, it helps explain why some

7:11

children are more at risk of becoming poor readers

7:13

than others. I'm also going to show

7:15

you that when kids do struggle, some of

7:17

them are more likely to get help. White

7:20

kids from families with money can often get

7:22

what they need. Those kids locked

7:24

up in Houston, almost all of them

7:26

are Black or Hispanic, and many of them were

7:28

once the struggling readers in their local public

7:31

schools who didn't get help. There

7:33

are a lot of students like that in schools all

7:35

over the country, including Nashville, Tennessee.

7:38

That's where we're going next to meet a woman named Visha

7:40

Hawkins. She was shocked to discover

7:43

just how far behind so many of the children

7:45

in her city actually

7:46

are.

7:51

Bisha Hawkins was a school system insider.

7:54

I was the system. Like, I

7:56

was a company girl. She was the liaison

7:59

between the director of the... Nashville

8:00

Public Schools and the elected school

8:02

board. She went to all the board meetings,

8:04

listened to every presentation about academic

8:06

performance for years. The

8:08

test scores were never very good.

8:10

But it was always couched in something,

8:13

right? Like it was always some kind of spin.

8:16

Test scores aren't good, but they're growing. Test

8:18

scores aren't good, but we're doing something about it.

8:21

And I never, I mean, quite honestly, like

8:23

I never just went to the website

8:25

and looked at the data myself, never.

8:29

until I left the district. That's when

8:31

it hit her. It was the fall of 2017. She'd

8:34

started writing about education. New

8:36

test scores had just been released, and she went online

8:39

to take a look. And I sat at my

8:41

desk

8:41

at home, and I mean, I was just crying.

8:44

Like, I could not believe we were doing

8:46

this to our children, and I couldn't

8:49

believe that I had missed it. The

8:51

test scores showed 86% of students from

8:54

low-income families were below grade level

8:56

in reading. Black and Hispanic students, 82%

8:59

of them were behind. We live

9:01

in a city, a great city, right? A beautiful

9:04

city, a growing city. Cranes

9:06

and construction crews were everywhere. We've

9:08

got cranes galore, and underneath

9:10

the cranes are

9:13

kids who cannot read. Unbelievable

9:15

to me. So I

9:17

decided to go on a little, I don't

9:21

know, research

9:23

tour. She started asking

9:25

people out for coffee. And so I had about 50

9:28

coffees with people, with

9:30

educators, administrators. And

9:33

she asked them, why are so many kids struggling

9:35

with reading? And every single person

9:37

I talked to, every single person I

9:39

talked to, except one, blamed

9:42

the parents for the reading crisis

9:46

in our city. They all said, parents

9:48

don't read enough to their children. Only

9:51

one person pointed to the schools. Everyone

9:53

else said, it's the

9:54

families, It's the home environment. It's poverty.

9:57

But that didn't sit right with Visha Hawkins. She

9:59

grew up. no one read to her, and

10:01

she learned to read. And then I thought, maybe

10:03

I'm asking the wrong people what

10:06

educator really is going to say, it's our

10:08

fault. You

10:10

know, we don't have the right curriculum. We don't,

10:12

you know, we didn't really learn how to teach reading in

10:15

college. Like, who's going to say that?

10:20

So she started talking to parents, and she

10:22

met Sonya Thomas. Sonya

10:24

is a founding member of a group called Nashville

10:26

PROPEL. PROPEL stands for Parents

10:29

Requiring Our Public Education System

10:31

to Lead. It was started by parents

10:33

whose local schools are on what is known as the

10:36

priority schools list. Priority

10:38

schools sounded good to Sonya Thomas until

10:40

she found out those are the schools with the lowest

10:43

test scores in the entire state,

10:45

the bottom 5 percent. Sonya

10:48

says many parents don't realize how

10:50

far behind their kids are. They don't

10:52

know that their children are

10:55

not reading at grade level and their children

10:57

truly don't know how to read. They

10:59

don't know until it costs them. This

11:01

is what happened with her youngest son, CJ.

11:04

The story starts in first grade.

11:07

I knew something was going on with him, but I could

11:09

not figure it out. He just didn't

11:11

seem to be getting it when it came to reading. The

11:14

school said he was behind, but nothing to worry

11:16

about. They were giving him extra help. There

11:18

was

11:18

never a conversation of He's

11:21

struggling with reading. It's, he needs

11:23

some intervention, so we're

11:25

gonna take him out of class, you know, read

11:27

with him a little more. I'm like, okay, great, good,

11:29

you know. She asked what she should be doing

11:32

at home, and the answer was, read to him. She

11:34

did, says she always had. But

11:36

things didn't seem to be getting better. Second

11:38

grade, third grade, fourth grade, Sonya

11:41

was really worried, but the school said

11:43

he was making progress. He did okay,

11:46

but I just knew that he wasn't

11:49

doing as well as my other

11:51

kids. So I started

11:52

asking myself, does he have

11:55

a learning disability? She asked for CJ

11:57

to be tested, but the school said no need. He

11:59

was fine.

12:00

She didn't know what to do. Tutoring,

12:02

private school, those weren't things she could

12:04

afford. She was desperate, and

12:06

she knew something about how CJ felt. She

12:09

had a hard time learning to read, and she says

12:11

no one helped her.

12:15

I don't remember being

12:18

taught to read. She just remembers

12:20

being expected to know how to do it.

12:22

As she got older, she says her problem wasn't that

12:24

she couldn't read the words. It's that she

12:26

didn't know what a lot of the words meant. Because if

12:29

I would read a sentence or read a passage, I'm

12:31

like, okay, what did that mean? She says she

12:33

was rarely assigned to read anything in school

12:35

except stuff in textbooks. No books,

12:37

no novels, no

12:40

any of that. Like, I did

12:42

not read books until

12:45

I actually got in high school in my English class

12:47

and we read Fahrenheit. Fahrenheit 451,

12:50

the 1950s dystopian novel by

12:53

Ray Bradbury. I hated that book because

12:55

that book was hard.

12:58

I didn't have the vocabulary. I

13:00

didn't have the understanding. It talked

13:03

around a whole bunch of things, and I just

13:05

did not understand. I could never

13:07

make the connection. And so I struggled. It

13:09

wasn't just that word stumped her when she was reading.

13:12

Words sometimes stumped her when people were talking,

13:14

too. She noticed it at work in

13:17

the healthcare industry. I mean, I work in a corporate

13:19

world, and I could tell sometimes when

13:22

they would have conversations, I didn't know

13:25

what they were talking about, and I would find myself

13:27

Googling words. It was embarrassing.

13:29

She did not want this to happen to her son. But

13:32

the schools kept telling her not to worry. His

13:34

grades were good. He was a well-behaved kid.

13:37

And then, seventh grade.

13:38

CJ had moved to a new school. It was September.

13:41

CJ's advisor called Sonya in for a meeting.

13:44

And the

13:46

advisor says that he's on task.

13:49

He has turned in all his assignments.

13:52

I'm like, yeah, I know. She

13:54

said, but when we tested him, He

13:57

reads on a second grade reading

13:59

level.

14:01

I lost it. It

14:03

felt like I cried for 15 minutes. I

14:07

sobbed. Eventually

14:09

she wiped the tears from her face, put her glasses

14:11

back on, and looked up. The

14:13

advisor told her the school would help CJ.

14:16

Sonya wanted to believe it, but

14:18

she'd been putting her faith in the school system for

14:21

years and this is where it had gotten

14:23

her,

14:23

a son in seventh grade at a second

14:25

grade reading level. from that

14:27

day on, I said, nobody

14:30

else will walk away feeling like that. No

14:33

child, no momma, no daddy. Like,

14:36

it's my life's work to make sure nobody

14:39

else feels like that. That

14:41

was when she helped start the parent group, Propel.

14:43

I love what they do. This is Visha

14:45

Hawkins again, the former school system insider.

14:48

When she met the parents of Propel, she realized

14:50

she was finally talking to the people she needed

14:52

to hear.

14:53

All those educator she'd had coffee

14:55

with, they blamed poverty for the

14:57

city's reading crisis and made it sound so unsolvable.

15:01

But after listening to parents like Sonya Thomas,

15:04

it all seemed much more urgent and clear.

15:07

We should be able to expect that

15:10

a kid goes

15:12

to school and learn

15:14

to read,

15:16

if nothing else.

15:22

In my years of reporting on reading, I haven't

15:24

met a teacher or a school administrator who

15:26

didn't want their students to be good readers.

15:29

But I've met a lot of educators who didn't know

15:32

what cognitive scientists have figured out about

15:34

how reading comprehension works. For

15:37

decades, those scientists have been studying what

15:39

is going on in our minds as we look at words

15:42

and make sense of text. And they've learned

15:44

some fascinating things. So

15:46

my name is Wes Hoover. I'm

15:49

a cognitive psychologist by training. I

15:51

am now retired after having worked in

15:53

the field for almost 40 years.

15:55

When Wes Hoover was in college in

15:57

the late 1960s, he got really interested in

15:59

the they did in language development just

16:02

how it worked how is

16:04

it possible that you are able

16:06

to learn a language just by being exposed

16:08

to it language just became

16:10

a fascination

16:11

and his interests and how people learn

16:13

to speak a language evolved into another

16:15

question when he was in graduate school how

16:17

does a person learn to read a language

16:20

in the nineteen seventies that was a controversial

16:23

question among academics there

16:25

were two big competing ideas

16:27

one of the ideas was there when

16:30

kids are reading a what they're trying

16:32

to do is complete comprehension

16:35

and the way they do it is to try

16:37

and get a flow going

16:39

about what meaning is being

16:42

communicated and reading and when

16:44

they come up on a word they don't recognize

16:47

to try and guess at what it is based

16:49

on the context of

16:51

of what they've read so far

16:54

the

16:54

idea was that as long as kids are focused

16:56

on the meaning of what they're reading they'll figure

16:58

out how to read the words this

17:00

view assume that learning how to read is similar

17:03

to learning how to talk that it happens

17:05

naturally through immersion the

17:07

other model is that know

17:09

reading a while it is focused

17:12

on comprehension getting the word off the

17:14

page actually is based

17:16

on analyzing the pieces of

17:18

the word doing what's called alphabetic

17:20

coding relating the letters to

17:23

the phonology of the language

17:25

the

17:25

teaching approach associated with this belief

17:27

was phonics teaching kids how

17:30

the sounds and words are represented by letters

17:32

the assumption was that kids need to be taught

17:34

how to read that it doesn't happen naturally

17:37

but

17:37

no one really knew how reading works

17:39

how do we even do it

17:42

when west hoover went to graduate school and nineteen

17:44

seventies he studied under a professor

17:47

who was trying to figure it out this

17:49

professor philip goff was trying to understand

17:51

that not just how we read but what's

17:53

going on when someone is having trouble reading

17:55

villas really trying to describe

17:58

reading disability what is it defines

18:01

whether someone can or can't read and what

18:03

are the categories of people that can't

18:05

read.

18:06

What Phil Goff knew was this. When

18:08

kids start school, the vast majority

18:11

of them are already quite good at speaking

18:13

their native language. The average

18:15

six-year-old, he wrote, has a mastery

18:17

of English that would be the envy of any college

18:19

graduate learning English as a second language.

18:22

But young children do not know how to read

18:25

most of the words they know how to say.

18:26

What happens when they come to school

18:28

is their language comprehension is fairly

18:30

high, and what they have to do is learn

18:33

word recognition. And so if they're

18:35

taught word recognition, then

18:37

they can read to the level at which

18:39

they can comprehend language.

18:41

The idea was that reading comprehension

18:43

has two parts. One is

18:46

your ability to understand meaning when

18:48

someone is talking or when text is read out

18:50

loud to you. That's language

18:52

comprehension. The other is your

18:54

ability to read printed words quickly

18:56

and accurately. That's word recognition.

18:59

If you can do both of those things, Phil Goff thought,

19:02

you can comprehend what you read. But

19:04

if you can only do one, or neither,

19:07

you can't. In 1986, he

19:10

and a colleague published a paper where they laid

19:12

out this model of reading comprehension. They

19:14

called it the simple view of reading.

19:16

The simple view does not say that reading

19:18

is simple.

19:19

It says that reading comprehension can be divided

19:22

into two parts.

19:23

Here's Wes Hoover again. If you

19:26

know someone's language comprehension

19:28

ability and their word recognition

19:30

ability, you will know how

19:32

well they read. You can predict perfectly their

19:34

reading comprehension. That's the hypothesis.

19:36

The hypothesis was first tested

19:38

and verified in a study that Wes Hoover

19:40

published with Phil Goff in 1990. The

19:43

basics of the model have been confirmed in

19:45

more than 150 studies since.

19:47

It's the big idea of

19:49

reading. That is reading

19:52

is complex. Word recognition

19:54

is complex. Language comprehension

19:56

is complex. But the big idea

19:59

of reading is that that if you can master those

20:01

two skills, those two complex skills,

20:04

then you can master reading comprehension.

20:06

When a person can't understand

20:08

what they read, according to the Simple View, they

20:10

have either a word recognition problem or

20:12

a language comprehension problem, or both.

20:15

Lots of struggling readers have both. That

20:18

was obvious when I was at the juvenile detention facility

20:20

in Houston.

20:22

One of the kids I met there was a 15-year-old I'll

20:24

call Mateo. I sat in on his

20:26

second reading lesson. Here he is trying

20:28

to sound out the word

20:29

toast. Mateo

20:38

is one of the kids who can barely read it all.

20:46

The

20:49

word is gloat. Sounding

20:52

it out is a first step. But does Mateo know

20:54

what the word gloat means? His tutor

20:56

asks him, Like, gloat?

20:57

Give me

21:00

something else. Like,

21:02

something shiny? Mateo doesn't

21:04

know what gloat means. His tutor tries

21:07

to define it, struggles a bit, then turns

21:09

to Jennifer Hunley, the assistant administrator,

21:11

who's in the corner keeping an eye on us. Like,

21:13

to

21:14

brag, like if someone just got their case

21:16

refused to have me and they're going, ooh,

21:18

I get to go home and no one else does. Yeah.

21:22

You know, kind of like that. Right. In

21:24

an ugly way. The

21:27

word gloat comes up again when Mateo is

21:29

trying to read a story called Taking

21:31

a Ride.

21:42

I'm not sure Mateo remembers the

21:44

meaning of that word. I'm not sure he

21:46

has any idea what he's reading. Listening

21:48

to him struggle through the text, I'm

21:50

having a hard time keeping track of what the story's

21:52

about. At the age of 15, Mateo

21:55

is a beginning reader.

21:57

This mental energy is still focused

21:59

on figuring out

22:00

how to sound out the words. There

22:04

are a few moments when he successfully pronounces

22:06

something and realizes it's a word he knows.

22:11

But many of the words, like gloat and

22:13

sneer and trait,

22:15

it was clear from earlier in the lesson that Mateo

22:18

didn't know the meaning of those words.

22:20

You could have read this story out loud to him and

22:22

he wouldn't have understood it all. Mateo

22:24

has a reading comprehension problem because

22:26

he has a hard time with both word recognition

22:29

and language comprehension. He's not

22:31

going to be a good reader until he gets better at both.

22:34

But if Mateo had learned how

22:36

to successfully sound out words earlier in

22:38

his life, he'd likely know the meaning

22:41

of a lot more words now.

22:43

Because there's a very powerful

22:45

thing in reading called Matthew

22:47

Effects. This is Wes Hoover again. It's

22:49

this idea that the rich get richer and the poor

22:51

get poorer.

22:52

It's a biblical reference. Here's how it

22:54

works. Let's say you enter

22:56

school and you get off to a good start when

22:59

it comes to the word recognition part of the

23:01

simple view of reading.

23:02

Then what happens is

23:04

that you tend to read more, you

23:06

tend to read more difficult texts,

23:09

you tend to engage in conversations

23:12

about those texts, and all

23:14

of those things then reciprocally

23:17

build your language comprehension

23:20

and your word recognition. Once you

23:22

start to be able to read and you read

23:25

more, the reading you

23:27

do further develops the

23:29

language comprehension and word recognition

23:31

skills

23:32

you have. That's the rich get richer. But

23:34

the opposite can happen. You don't get off

23:36

to a good start with word recognition, either

23:38

because it's something that's really hard for you. For

23:41

example, you have dyslexia, which is characterized

23:43

by difficulty with discerning the sounds and words.

23:47

Or, you don't get off to a good start with word recognition

23:49

because no one teaches it to you, or

23:51

both. It's hard for you and

23:54

you're not taught how to do

23:54

it. kids who can't

23:57

read very well will

23:59

start not

24:00

reading very much at all, they'll

24:02

try and read less complex

24:05

texts, they'll get frustrated

24:08

and stop reading altogether, and

24:11

that will have the effect of not moving

24:14

either their word recognition or

24:16

language comprehension skills

24:17

forward.

24:20

When kids don't get the instruction they need,

24:23

they can easily grow into adulthood without

24:25

knowing basic things about how written

24:27

language works, like Mateo and

24:29

Dachon. I don't know what happened to

24:31

them.

24:32

The study they're part of is still going on. Struggling

24:34

readers in the juvenile detention system in Houston

24:37

continue to get tutoring. But not

24:39

at the facility I visited in 2018.

24:41

shut down last year as part of

24:43

an effort to lock up fewer kids.

24:55

You're listening to What the Words Say from

24:57

APM Reports. I'm Emily Hanford.

25:01

Studies show that almost all children can become

25:03

readers. They have the cognitive capacity

25:05

to do it.

25:07

But a lot of them aren't becoming readers.

25:10

The National Assessment of Educational Progress

25:12

shows that roughly half of Black and Hispanic

25:14

children and nearly a quarter of white

25:16

kids do not have basic reading

25:18

comprehension skills by fourth grade.

25:21

A lot of those children's parents have been told,

25:23

don't worry, your child will catch up. But

25:26

most of them won't catch up. Coming

25:28

up, we'll hear about why so many kids like

25:30

Mateo and Deshaun are not getting the

25:32

instruction they need. I go into poor

25:35

schools, nobody has dyslexia in a poor

25:37

school, in the face of

25:39

a population where eight

25:42

and a half out of 10 are

25:44

struggling with reading, who has a

25:47

reading disability? The answer

25:49

is we have no idea.

25:51

Support for APM Reports comes from the Spencer

25:54

Foundation, Lumina Foundation, and the Hollyhock

25:56

Foundation. More in a minute, this is

25:58

APM American Public...

26:00

media.

26:02

Hey, it's Emily. I've been covering

26:04

the way reading is taught in the United States

26:07

for the past five years. There's

26:09

a lot of money and power wrapped up in reading

26:11

programs. And as an investigative

26:13

journalist, I have a very different kind of power.

26:16

I have a commitment to the public to uncover

26:18

injustice, bring it to light, and

26:21

hold the powerful accountable. sold

26:24

a story and other journalism like this

26:26

with a donation today at soldastory.org-donate

26:31

or click the link in our show notes. In 1996,

26:35

one law changed welfare in the

26:37

US by adding work requirements

26:40

and letting for-profit companies run

26:42

welfare offices. What happened

26:44

next is complicated. Government

26:46

is one of your customers, businesses are

26:49

another. What about the welfare recipient?

26:51

Are they? I think of the mores the product

26:53

of our company. They're our inventory.

26:56

This season of The Uncertain Hour, we uncover

26:58

how for-profit companies cash in on

27:00

public benefits. Listen to The Uncertain

27:02

Hour wherever you get your podcasts.

27:05

Welcome

27:05

back. I'm Emily Hanford. And this

27:08

is What the Words Say from APM Reports.

27:10

Good morning. Thank

27:12

you for being our guinea pigs today in

27:15

our simulation.

27:16

We're in an elementary school classroom in Harrisburg,

27:18

Pennsylvania. There are about 20 teachers

27:21

seated at tables. A consultant

27:23

named Michael Hunter is taking them through an exercise

27:25

to demonstrate some of the things scientists

27:28

have figured out about how reading works.

27:30

I need four brave volunteers.

27:33

A few hands go up. Michael sends them into

27:35

the hallway. Before I tell you what happens

27:37

next, I want to go back to the two big ideas

27:40

about how people read that academics

27:42

were arguing about back in the 1970s.

27:47

One idea was that readers use the meaning

27:49

of what they're reading to predict what the words

27:51

will be. Learning to read is

27:53

not about sounding words out, it's about

27:55

using context to guess what the words

27:58

are. According to this theory,

28:00

readers don't necessarily have to read

28:02

every word accurately to comprehend what they're

28:04

reading. The other theory was

28:06

that learning to read is a process of looking carefully

28:09

at words, sounding them out, and matching

28:11

those pronunciations with words you know in

28:13

spoken language. If you can't

28:15

accurately identify the words, your comprehension

28:18

will suffer. That's the idea

28:20

that decades of scientific research has confirmed,

28:23

and that's what Michael Hunter wants to demonstrate

28:25

with the teachers in Harrisburg.

28:26

So let's bring in our first

28:29

reader. A first grade teacher named

28:31

Katie comes in from the hall. Michael

28:33

projects a passage onto a screen at the front of the

28:35

room and asks her to read it out loud. A

28:37

tragnex is a simple jeed

28:41

used for finding plivons. Most

28:44

will fit in your Brisbane. 30% of

28:47

the words in this passage are nonsense words. Katie

28:49

does her best to sound them out, but she has no

28:51

idea what they mean. When she's done,

28:54

Michael asks her some questions.

28:56

First question is, what tool is the topic

28:58

of the passage? Tragnexs. Okay. Why

29:01

is the face of a clock mentioned?

29:03

I have absolutely

29:06

no clue. Okay. Katie

29:08

just demonstrated what reading comprehension

29:10

is like when you're faced with a bunch of words

29:12

you don't know.

29:13

The next volunteer comes in from the hall

29:15

to read the same passage, but this time fewer

29:18

of the words are nonsense, just 20%. This

29:21

is Jalissa. Tragnix is

29:23

a simple tool used for fighting plivons.

29:26

Most will fit in your palm. The face is similar

29:29

to the... There are two kinds of nonsense words in

29:31

this passage to demonstrate an important

29:33

point about decoding. Some

29:35

of the words are hard for Jelissa to sound

29:37

out. Plivons, for example, P-L-I-Y-V-N-S.

29:43

She hesitates and stumbles on that one because

29:45

English words aren't spelled that way. She's

29:48

not sure how to decode it and she doesn't know

29:50

what it means. A word like Tragnex,

29:53

that's pretty easy for her to decode. But

29:55

decoding doesn't help much because Jelissa doesn't

29:57

know what the word means. the point.

30:00

is you can sound like a decent reader

30:02

if you have good decoding skills,

30:04

but it doesn't necessarily mean you understand

30:06

what you're reading. How was Jelissa's

30:08

comprehension when she didn't know 20% of the words? Tragic.

30:12

It was better than mine. So

30:15

when do the benefits of context kick in? At

30:18

what point can you figure out what the words say

30:20

from the meaning of what you're reading? The

30:22

next reader comes in from the

30:24

the hall. Read aloud to us. Do your best reading.

30:27

Now just 10% of the words are nonsense.

30:29

A track next is a simple tool used

30:32

for finding directions. This reader

30:34

figures out what the passage is about.

30:36

How do you feel about your comprehension? Did you

30:38

totally understand the passage? I did not

30:40

until I got about halfway and then realized what I was talking

30:43

about. And then I started to comprehend it. She

30:46

knew enough of the words to get a gist of what

30:48

was going on and then it clicked. It's

30:50

about a compass.

30:52

But she already knows what a compass

30:55

is and how it works. She was able to

30:57

fill in the gaps left by the handful of words

30:59

she didn't know by relying on her background

31:01

knowledge.

31:02

This happens all the time in reading.

31:05

Even when you can easily read all the words,

31:08

your comprehension can be aided or impeded,

31:11

depending on what you already know about the topic.

31:12

Easy example is if

31:15

I don't know if you know cricket. This is reading researcher

31:17

Wes Hoover again, and I don't know anything about

31:20

cricket, except that it's a bat and ball game,

31:22

not played much in the United States.

31:23

If you read a sports

31:26

column about cricket, you most

31:28

likely would have great difficulty understanding

31:30

it as opposed to a column written about baseball.

31:32

This assumes I know something about baseball,

31:35

and I do. Probably more than a typical

31:37

kid growing up in New Zealand, for example, where

31:40

there's a lot of cricket but not much baseball. So

31:42

kids in New Zealand can quickly

31:44

understand accounts of cricket matches,

31:47

but they have great difficulty understanding

31:49

accounts of baseball matches. And

31:51

the problem is they don't have the background knowledge to

31:54

interpret what's going

31:55

on. Your ability to comprehend what

31:57

you read is linked to your knowledge. This

31:59

is one.

32:00

reason there's an association between a child's

32:02

reading comprehension and their family's income.

32:05

More income often means more opportunity

32:07

for experiences that build knowledge of

32:09

the world.

32:10

The teacher who figured out tragnex

32:12

meant compass already knew something about

32:15

compasses.

32:16

If you don't know anything about compasses, one way to

32:18

learn about them is through reading. But

32:20

your chances of learning something about

32:22

compasses through reading will be impeded

32:25

if you can't read the words.

32:27

That's why teaching kids how to read words

32:29

is so important.

32:30

I have a master's degree in reading and I didn't

32:33

learn this. Lisa Flute is a reading

32:35

specialist in Harrisburg who participated

32:37

in the demonstration we just heard. It's

32:39

part of a year-long professional development series

32:41

on what scientists have discovered about how

32:44

reading works and how to apply that to teaching.

32:47

Lisa Flute says she didn't learn about the science

32:49

of reading in her preparation to be a teacher. She

32:51

learned that idea from the 1970s. goal

32:54

is meaning, meaning, meaning. What she didn't

32:56

understand is how kids get to meaning.

32:59

She didn't spend much time teaching kids how to

33:01

decode words because she didn't think it was necessary.

33:04

They had other ways to get the meaning. She

33:06

now realizes what a mistake that was.

33:09

Some of her students needed much more help.

33:12

And there are kids that I'm picturing my mind right now

33:14

that I want to say I'm sorry to.

33:25

I've talked to a lot of teachers who express

33:27

regret about what they didn't know. For

33:30

many of them, the simple view of reading is

33:32

a big aha moment. They didn't

33:34

fully appreciate the importance of word recognition,

33:37

and they didn't quite get how the language comprehension

33:40

part works either. Language

33:42

comprehension is critical. Research

33:44

shows that once children have mastered the basics

33:47

of decoding, their ability to understand

33:49

what they read is largely determined

33:51

by their oral language skills, their knowledge,

33:54

and their vocabulary.

33:55

A large body of research

33:57

shows that children from low-income families

33:59

come in into

34:00

school knowing the meaning of far fewer

34:02

words on average than higher income

34:04

kids. This can put them at a disadvantage

34:07

at the outset because making sense of what you're

34:09

reading is about matching what you see in print

34:11

with what you already know in spoken language. This

34:14

also means that if the language you speak at home

34:17

is different from the language you use in school,

34:19

learning to read is likely to take more time

34:22

and maybe more challenging. This

34:24

is true for English language learners, kids

34:26

who speak Spanish or Korean or Arabic at

34:28

home. It can also be

34:30

true for children who are native English speakers.

34:33

Julie Washington studies language and reading

34:35

development in African American children. She's

34:37

specifically interested in the role of African

34:40

American English.

34:42

African American English is a dialect of English.

34:45

Every language has dialects. They're variations

34:47

of a parent language, different ways of pronouncing

34:49

words, and different vocabulary and grammar, too.

34:52

So an example of

34:53

African American English is

34:56

one day me and my mom was

34:59

at home. That is completely acceptable

35:01

in African American English. There was a moment

35:03

when Julie Washington realized that children

35:05

who come into school speaking African American

35:08

English might have a harder time learning

35:10

how to read. This was way back in the beginning

35:12

of my career. Worked with a four

35:14

year old and we were reading Are

35:17

You My Mother? by P.D. Eastman.

35:19

Today, we're going to read a story about a

35:21

little bird looking for his mother. And

35:24

so the baby bird jumps out of the nest,

35:26

goes to different animals,

35:29

objects, and says, Are you my mother? Are

35:31

you my mother? He said

35:34

to the hen.

35:35

No, said the hen. And

35:38

so it's this, Are you my mother? I am

35:40

not a, that goes through the story. So

35:43

when this little girl, African American

35:45

dialect speaker, goes to retell

35:47

the story to me. She says, is

35:50

you my mama? I ain't none of your

35:52

mama.

35:53

I laughed. It was hilarious and it was

35:55

fun. But then I went back to

35:57

my office and I thought about what she

35:59

had to

36:00

do in order to listen

36:02

to this story that was told in

36:05

a language form that she doesn't actually

36:07

use. She recoded it into

36:09

her own dialect and then she

36:12

told me the story. That takes a lot

36:14

of working memory.

36:15

It takes a pretty good vocabulary.

36:18

That little girl had to do a lot of work because

36:21

there was a difference between the language she

36:23

knew and the language of the book. The kid who

36:25

comes to school whose language system mirrors

36:28

the book, doesn't have that work to do. The

36:31

kid who looks at the book, it's

36:33

exactly

36:33

the same system he uses, can go

36:35

straight for decoding and not have

36:37

to do all those other steps in between.

36:40

Julie Washington says schools need to understand

36:43

that children who are heavy dialect users may

36:45

need more time and more help to

36:47

be successful with reading. She says

36:50

almost all low-income African American

36:52

children use African American English

36:54

at home.

36:55

Middle-income kids are more

36:58

likely to either not use it at all or

37:00

to be able to code switch. Because they've

37:03

had more access

37:04

outside of the community, they

37:06

go to schools where there are more kids who

37:08

are using mainstream English, they are more

37:10

likely to be able to code switch in and out.

37:18

Think

37:18

of family income as a kind of buffer

37:20

when it comes to the risk of being a struggling

37:23

reader. The more resources your

37:25

family has, the more opportunities you're

37:27

likely to have for early life experiences

37:29

that tilt things in your favor when it comes

37:31

to learning how to read.

37:33

But it's not just how affluent your

37:35

family is, it's how affluent your school

37:37

is, too. High poverty schools

37:40

are less effective, on average, when it comes

37:42

to promoting reading achievement. And

37:44

according to the U.S. Department of Education, nearly

37:47

half of all black students in this country go

37:49

to high poverty schools, nearly half

37:51

of all Hispanic kids too.

37:53

White kids? 88% of

37:56

them go to schools where most students are

37:58

from low-income families.

38:01

And here's the thing. If you're a struggling

38:03

reader and you go to a school where most

38:05

of the students are from low-income families,

38:08

your problems with reading may go unnoticed.

38:11

Because a lot of the other kids are probably having

38:13

a hard time learning to read, too.

38:15

Here's Julie Washington again. I go into

38:17

poor schools. Nobody has dyslexia in

38:19

a poor school. in the face

38:22

of a population where 8.5

38:25

out of 10

38:27

are struggling with reading,

38:28

who has a reading disability?

38:31

The answer is we have no idea.

38:33

She says part of the problem is the way federal

38:35

law defines learning disabilities.

38:38

The law says a child cannot

38:40

qualify for a learning disability if

38:43

that child's learning problems are primarily

38:45

the result of economic disadvantage.

38:48

So what that policy is saying, we've

38:50

decided as a country that if

38:52

you are having

38:53

trouble reading and you're poor, you're

38:56

having trouble with reading because you're poor, because

38:58

our policy does not allow you to be both

39:01

learning disabled and poor.

39:03

The goal was to prevent low-income kids of color

39:06

from being over-identified for special education,

39:09

but the policy has had unintended consequences.

39:12

We

39:12

hear from teachers that they

39:14

have been told not to refer any

39:16

more children of color, that they're already at their

39:19

threshold.

39:20

This is Paul Morgan, a professor at

39:22

Penn State.

39:23

His research shows that if you look at children

39:25

having the hardest time with reading, kids

39:28

who score in the bottom 10%, you

39:31

find that white children are much more

39:33

likely to be receiving special education

39:35

services than children of color.

39:37

He says there are likely a number of

39:39

things going on. Part of it is expectations.

39:42

The white child struggling must have a disability,

39:45

whereas the black child struggling is just

39:47

struggling, like so many other kids in her

39:49

school.

39:50

And then there's the fact that getting special

39:52

education services for a child with a reading

39:55

disability can be difficult, no matter

39:57

what kind of school the child goes to.

39:59

Too often. I think parents have

40:01

to fight. And when the school

40:03

says no, there's

40:06

not much of a recourse for the parent to

40:08

engage in short of legal action, which

40:10

is very costly.

40:11

It's a system that favors people

40:13

with money.

40:14

Some parents spend thousands of dollars

40:17

trying to get their kids into special ed.

40:20

But a child who is having a hard time learning

40:22

to read doesn't necessarily have a learning

40:24

disability. Paul Morgan points

40:26

to the experience of his own two kids.

40:28

Our oldest is

40:30

a voracious reader and took to

40:33

it readily. He

40:35

seemed to benefit from what

40:37

our local school did in terms

40:40

of teaching and reading.

40:40

This wasn't the case with his younger son.

40:43

He really was starting

40:45

to experience difficulties

40:48

fairly early by kindergarten

40:51

first grade.

40:51

The school's advice to Paul and his wife? Read

40:54

story books to him, surround him with

40:56

books. books. But they'd been reading to him

40:58

since he was a baby. They had tons

41:01

of books in their home. Language

41:03

comprehension wasn't the issue. Paul's

41:06

son needed to be taught how to read words,

41:08

so he and his wife started doing that.

41:10

We were in a position to reorganize our work

41:12

schedules,

41:14

and we, just every

41:16

morning before we went to his classroom,

41:19

set aside 10-15 minutes of regular

41:22

practice and then

41:24

he was okay. Things made sense

41:26

to him. He was decoding

41:29

and starting to read quickly and fluently

41:32

and that that was what he needed.

41:33

They caught the problem and were able

41:35

to fix it pretty easily. That's

41:37

not going to be the case with every child. Some

41:39

kids will need lots of instruction but

41:41

intervening early is critical.

41:44

If you can't read well in

41:46

the early grades, your Your peers

41:48

notice, your teacher notices, you

41:50

notice, and it really starts to have

41:54

negative consequences on your social-emotional

41:56

development and your behavior.

41:58

most children who are struggling.

42:00

with reading at the end of first grade don't

42:02

catch up. Because

42:04

the kids who got off to a good start in reading

42:06

are catapulting ahead.

42:09

Those good readers are soon able to read

42:11

everything they know how to say. And

42:13

now, because they can read lots of words,

42:16

they're gaining knowledge and teaching themselves

42:18

the meaning of new words through reading.

42:21

That's the rich get richer. When kids

42:23

struggle, they tend to read less and miss

42:25

out on tons of little opportunities to learn

42:27

through reading. All those

42:30

missed opportunities add up. One

42:32

study estimated that a fifth grader who was

42:34

a good reader, at the 90th percentile

42:37

compared to her peers, encounters

42:39

almost two million words

42:41

in text every year just in stuff

42:43

she reads outside of school. The

42:46

average child who reads at the 10th percentile

42:48

encounters just 8,000 words

42:51

outside of school.

42:51

Think about that. And

42:54

then think about a kid who gets to seventh

42:56

grade reading on a second grade level.

42:59

That's what Sonya Thomas was told about her son.

43:02

What happened with CJ?

43:05

Hello. Hello. So, I'm

43:07

Emily. I'm CJ.

43:10

I never got a chance to meet CJ in person.

43:13

The coronavirus abruptly canceled travel

43:15

while I was reporting this story. So

43:17

I met him on Zoom with his mom. I

43:20

asked him what he remembers about being taught to read.

43:22

much he says except that it was hard.

43:25

What was hard? What was

43:27

it about it that was hard? Do you know? Saying

43:30

the words out loud and reading out loud. Reading

43:33

out loud. Could you sound

43:35

them out and say the words and

43:38

then you didn't know what they meant or did you have a hard

43:40

time just sounding them out? Both.

43:44

Both. Do you

43:46

remember anyone teaching you how

43:48

to sound out words? No. No,

43:52

but maybe they did and you don't remember. Yeah.

43:55

Sonya had warned me that CJ isn't much of

43:58

a talker, so I wasn't surprised by his

44:00

one-word answers. He's being 13,

44:02

don't want to do it. I gotcha.

44:05

My big question about CJ's reading is this.

44:08

Does

44:08

he have a disability that the school system

44:11

missed or is the problem that CJ

44:13

was never taught how to read or both?

44:16

His mom wants to know the answers to those

44:18

questions too.

44:24

So,

44:24

Sonya requested all of CJ's school

44:26

records and APM reports hired

44:28

a professor named Zach Barnes to review

44:30

those records.

44:31

I'm assistant professor of special education

44:33

at Austin P. State University in Clarksville,

44:35

Tennessee. Before that, Zach was a special

44:37

education teacher in the Nashville schools, so

44:40

he's familiar with the forms and assessments in

44:42

CJ's file.

44:43

Sonya and I met with Zach virtually,

44:45

and he went through what he found in CJ's records,

44:48

starting in kindergarten.

44:49

From the data that we're seeing, CJ

44:51

was starting off behind. The records

44:54

are sort of frustrating, though. They don't say what

44:56

he was behind in, just that he was below

44:58

benchmark. When CJ

45:00

started first grade, he took a reading test

45:03

that placed him at the 24th percentile

45:05

nationally. That means more than

45:07

three quarters of first graders in the country

45:10

were doing better than he was. There's

45:12

a form in the file that says CJ had no

45:14

problem understanding and using vocabulary,

45:17

but that he spoke slowly. Samia

45:19

noticed this too. I do

45:20

remember me having some concerns

45:23

about his speech and

45:27

him being really shy, like not

45:29

talking a lot.

45:30

There's no indication

45:32

CJ was evaluated for a speech issue

45:34

or a reading problem, but there is

45:37

a handwritten note Sonia wrote when CJ

45:39

was in first grade, asking the school

45:41

to test him for a learning disability. At

45:44

the end of first grade, CJ took

45:46

the same assessment he took at the beginning of the year,

45:49

the one that showed he read at the 24th percentile.

45:52

This time, he scored at the 12th percentile.

45:55

That means nearly 90% of kids his age

45:57

were doing better than he was.

46:00

Sonya tears up when Zach points this

46:02

out.

46:03

Sorry, but this tears me at all

46:05

the pieces.

46:07

We spend nearly two hours going

46:09

over CJ's entire school file,

46:12

grade by grade. There are nearly 200

46:15

pages of records, and Zach notices

46:17

a pattern. Some years, CJ

46:19

got pulled out of the classroom for extra help with

46:21

reading. His test scores went up.

46:23

Then the help stopped.

46:25

I asked Zach later if this is unusual. he

46:27

said no, and not just in Nashville, in

46:29

lots of schools. He likened it

46:31

to a lifeguard saving someone and

46:33

then allowing them to drown a few minutes later.

46:37

Things might have been different for CJ if he'd

46:39

been in special education. He would

46:41

have had an individualized education program

46:43

and rights to services protected by federal

46:46

law.

46:46

But to get into special ed, you

46:48

need to be identified with a disability.

46:51

Zach says to determine if CJ has a disability,

46:53

he'd need a full evaluation from a school

46:56

psychologist.

46:57

CJ never got one of those.

46:59

Zach says he should have.

47:01

He's this kind of student that we really

47:03

need to dig deep on to

47:05

figure out how

47:07

can we help CJ? Zach

47:09

offers to help Sonya get CJ an evaluation.

47:13

Sonya is grateful, but angry.

47:15

Her son just finished eighth grade. She

47:18

asked for him to be tested for a learning disability

47:21

in first grade. She wonders

47:23

how many other kids needed help and

47:25

didn't get it. There's

47:27

this heavy feeling

47:29

that I have of so

47:34

many people that's not going to get it and worse

47:36

off than him. And I don't know what to do

47:39

except to keep telling parents to question everything

47:41

and everybody so they don't have to go all

47:44

of these years like I did to try to get

47:46

down to the bottom of it.

47:47

I contacted the Nashville Public Schools to

47:49

see if someone can answer questions about what happened

47:52

with CJ. a spokesperson declined

47:54

to comment.

48:00

There are kids like CJ all over

48:02

the country. Learning to read does

48:04

not come easily to them. Schools

48:06

tell their parents, read to him, he'll be okay.

48:09

But he's not. Some

48:11

kids get help. Their parents pay for

48:14

it, or they teach their child themselves. Or

48:16

the child gets into special education, where

48:18

he's more likely to get the kind of instruction he needs.

48:21

But if your child is not learning to read

48:23

in school, and you don't have the money or

48:25

time to deal with it yourself, what

48:28

do you do? The equity

48:30

implications of this are stunning. A

48:32

child from a low or even a moderate

48:34

income family who is having a hard time learning

48:36

to read may never get what he

48:39

needs to become a good reader.

48:45

There

48:45

are several ways to view what's going

48:47

on with reading in this country.

48:49

One is to see it as a special education problem.

48:51

We have lots of kids with learning disabilities

48:53

who aren't getting the help they need.

48:56

We do,

48:56

but that isn't the whole story. A third

48:59

of fourth graders in this country can't read

49:01

on a basic level. They can't all

49:03

need special education.

49:05

Remember Paul Morgan's son?

49:07

He got the help he needed and he was fine.

49:10

He's doing well academically, about to start

49:12

high school, the same age as CJ.

49:15

Another popular explanation

49:17

is poverty. Kids can't read because

49:19

they're hungry, they're stressed, they weren't read

49:21

to enough at home. Poverty

49:23

plays a role, no question. There's

49:25

lots of research on this. But children

49:28

from low-income families can learn to read

49:30

well, and when they do, it can change

49:33

their lives. Visha Hawkins

49:35

grew up poor, she learned to read, and

49:37

now she has a master's degree.

49:39

A third explanation is the tests

49:42

themselves. They're not measuring reading ability

49:44

accurately. The levels are set too high.

49:47

Reasonable people can disagree

49:49

on how proficiency levels are set on standardized

49:51

tests, and no test will be able to

49:53

measure everyone's reading ability accurately. For

49:56

example, if you're a kid who doesn't know anything about

49:58

cricket, and there's There's a passage about... cricket on

50:00

your fourth grade reading test, you may not

50:02

do so well. Maybe you would have done better

50:04

if the passage was about baseball.

50:06

But arguing about the tests

50:09

misses the big picture.

50:10

Many kids are struggling, and there are

50:12

parents like Sonya Thomas crying out for

50:15

help all over this country.

50:21

What I've learned from my years of reporting

50:23

on this topic is that a big part of the problem

50:25

is many kids aren't being taught how

50:28

to read. Old assumptions

50:30

about how reading works are pervasive in

50:32

schools. The idea that readers

50:34

don't need to sound out words, they can use

50:36

context instead. The idea that

50:39

kids who are behind will catch up. The

50:41

idea that learning to read is like learning to talk,

50:43

that it happens through exposure. It doesn't.

50:46

Cognitive scientists have known this for a long

50:49

time. Phil Goff, the guy who

50:51

came up with a simple view of reading, published

50:53

a paper in 1980 called Learning

50:55

to Read, an Unnatural Act. He

50:57

wrote this, the statistically

51:00

average child normally endowed and

51:02

normally taught

51:03

learns to read only with considerable difficulty.

51:06

He does not learn to read naturally.

51:09

The bottom line is that learning to read is not

51:12

easy for many kids. Reading difficulty

51:15

is natural. And a lot of kids

51:17

are not being taught what they need to know. Visha

51:21

Hawkins wants to see a movement of parents

51:23

demanding better reading instruction.

51:26

I mean, I just envision, like,

51:28

just thousands of parents

51:30

to send it upon central office or

51:32

the courthouse. You know, just force

51:35

people to look at the kids,

51:37

to look at the families that's not being

51:40

served. I mean, y'all are taking our

51:42

tax dollars, but

51:44

we're not getting a return on that investment.

51:47

Sonya Thomas wants to see a movement, too.

51:49

Why isn't everyone

51:51

in this country angry

51:55

like me? Why are they

51:57

not losing sleep? It's

52:00

unacceptable for children

52:03

to not have a chance right off the

52:05

bat. And

52:08

I'm not going to let anybody sleep. We

52:10

are not going to let anybody sleep until

52:13

we have changed and changed

52:16

for the better for all children.

52:19

Sonya is now executive director of PROPEL,

52:21

the parent group she helped found. It's her

52:23

full-time job.

52:25

And she's determined to make sure that all the CJs

52:28

and Mateos and Dachans out there get what

52:30

they need to learn how to read.

52:39

You've

52:39

been listening to What the Words Say from

52:42

APM Reports. It was produced by

52:44

me, Emily Hanford, and edited by

52:46

Catherine Winter. Research and production

52:48

help from Sabby Robinson and John Hernandez.

52:52

Our associate producer is Alex Baumhart.

52:55

editors are Dave Mann and Andy Cruz.

52:58

The final mix was by Chris Julin and Craig

53:00

Thorsen. Fact-checking by Betsy

53:02

Towner Levine. The APM

53:04

Reports team includes Sasha Eslanian

53:06

and Lauren Humpert. Our editor-in-chief

53:09

is Chris Worthington.

53:09

Special thanks to Stephen

53:12

Smith and Shelley Lankford.

53:16

If

53:16

you go to our website, APMreports.org,

53:18

you can find a version of this story with lots

53:21

of links to books and articles where you can read

53:23

more about the simple view of reading and other

53:25

research referred to in this program. You

53:28

can find all of the reporting we've done on reading

53:30

at a special collections page, APMreports.org

53:33

slash reading, and on our podcast,

53:35

Educate. Support for APM

53:38

Reports comes from the Spencer Foundation, Lumina

53:40

Foundation, and the Holly Hawk Foundation. This

53:43

is APM American Public Media.

53:51

This is Emily again. You've been listening to

53:54

What the Words Say from 2020. A bonus episode

53:56

of the Sold a Story

53:59

podcast is...

54:00

coming soon. You can go to our

54:02

website for more about Sold a Story.

54:04

It's soldastory.org.

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features